To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Speech disfluencies.

Journal articles on the topic 'Speech disfluencies'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Speech disfluencies.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Plexico, Laura W., Julie E. Cleary, Ashlynn McAlpine, and Allison M. Plumb. "Disfluency Characteristics Observed in Young Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Preliminary Report." Perspectives on Fluency and Fluency Disorders 20, no. 2 (2010): 42–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/ffd20.2.42.

Full text
Abstract:
This descriptive study evaluates the speech disfluencies of 8 verbal children between 3 and 5 years of age with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Speech samples were collected for each child during standardized interactions. Percentage and types of disfluencies observed during speech samples are discussed. Although they did not have a clinical diagnosis of stuttering, all of the young children with ASD in this study produced disfluencies. In addition to stuttering-like disfluencies and other typical disfluencies, the children with ASD also produced atypical disfluencies, which usually are not o
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Furini, Juliana, Luana Altran Picoloto, Eduarda Marconato, Anelise Junqueira Bohnen, Ana Claudia Vieira Cardoso, and Cristiane Moço Canhetti de Oliveira. "The role of auditory temporal cues in the fluency of stuttering adults." Revista CEFAC 19, no. 5 (2017): 611–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620171954417.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACT Purpose: to compare the frequency of disfluencies and speech rate in spontaneous speech and reading in adults with and without stuttering in non-altered and delayed auditory feedback (NAF, DAF). Methods: participants were 30 adults: 15 with Stuttering (Research Group - RG), and 15 without stuttering (Control Group - CG). The procedures were: audiological assessment and speech fluency evaluation in two listening conditions, normal and delayed auditory feedback (100 milliseconds delayed by Fono Tools software). Results: the DAF caused a significant improvement in the fluency of spontane
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Temple, Liz. "Disfluencies in learner speech." Australian Review of Applied Linguistics 15, no. 2 (1992): 29–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/aral.15.2.03tem.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Disfluent phenomena such as pauses, hesitations and repairs, are investigated in a corpus of forty-two short samples of spontaneous speech of native French speakers and learners of French. Significant quantitative differences between native speakers and learners were found and interpreted in the light of Bialystok’s and McLaughlin’s theories of language processing and the two key concepts of automaticity and control. A greater requirement of processing time on the part of the language learners was attributed to their greater use of controlled processes. An analysis of lexical and synt
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Gordon, Pearl A., and Harold L. Luper. "Speech disfluencies in nonstutterers." Journal of Fluency Disorders 14, no. 6 (1989): 429–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730x(89)90029-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Myers, Florence L., Klaas Bakker, Kenneth O. St. Louis, and Lawrence J. Raphael. "Disfluencies in cluttered speech." Journal of Fluency Disorders 37, no. 1 (2012): 9–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2011.10.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Boughaba, Hicham. "Speech Disfluencies in Simultaneous Interpretations of Spontaneous and Non-Spontaneous Speeches." International Journal of Linguistics, Literature and Translation 4, no. 7 (2021): 15–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.32996/ijllt.2021.4.7.3.

Full text
Abstract:
The present paper is an attempt to examine speech disfluencies in simultaneous interpretations of spontaneous and non-spontaneous speeches from English into Arabic. It focuses on the difference between the rate of disfluencies in renditions of spontaneous and non-spontaneous speeches. The data were collected from authentic sessions of professional interpreters from English into Arabic. The speeches and interpretations were transcribed. The data was divided into two different categories: ‘spontaneous source speeches and their renditions’ and ‘non-spontaneous speeches and their renditions’. Disf
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Półrola, Paweł J., and Jolanta Góral-Półrola. "Speech disfluencies in Parkinson’s disease." Medical Studies 4 (2015): 267–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.5114/ms.2015.56668.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Lease, M., M. Johnson, and E. Charniak. "Recognizing disfluencies in conversational speech." IEEE Transactions on Audio, Speech and Language Processing 14, no. 5 (2006): 1566–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/tasl.2006.878269.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Hubbard, Carol P., and Ehud Yairi. "Clustering of Disfluencies in the Speech of Stuttering and Nonstuttering Preschool Children." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 31, no. 2 (1988): 228–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3102.228.

Full text
Abstract:
Cluster formation in speech disfluencies of 15 preschool stuttering children and an equal number of nonstuttering control subjects was studied and compared. Clustering was defined as the occurrence of two or more disfluencies on the same or adjacent words. A computer program, based on the Monte Carlo method, was used to generate sets of expected numbers of single disfluencies and disfluencies in various cluster sizes according to simulation models. Results indicate that disfluencies occurred in clusters more often than could have been expected by chance for both groups. The proportions of clus
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Befi-Lopes, Debora Maria, Ana Manhani Cáceres-Assenço, Suellen Fernanda Marques, and Marcely Vieira. "School-age children with specific language impairment produce more speech disfluencies than their peers." CoDAS 26, no. 6 (2014): 439–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20142014095.

Full text
Abstract:
PURPOSE: To compare the occurrence of speech disfluencies during narrative production in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and their age-matched peers. METHODS: The study included 60 children aged between 7 and 10 years, 40 with typical language development and 20 with SLI. For data collection, a series of 15 stories was used, each one represented by pictures composed of four scenes. Narratives were transcripted and the speech disfluencies presented on them were classified as stuttering-like disfluencies (part-word repetition, single-syllable word repetition, and dysrhythmic pho
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

KHEYRANDISH, MOHAMMAD, SAEED SETAYESHI, and AMIR MASOUD RAHMANI. "DISFLUENCY DETECTION ON SPONTANEOUS SPEECH USING LEARNING CELLULAR AUTOMATA." International Journal on Artificial Intelligence Tools 21, no. 05 (2012): 1250020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/s0218213012500200.

Full text
Abstract:
Speech recognition is an efficient tool for interacting between human and machines; but the speech includes some undesirable components (disfluencies), sometimes, and is called spontaneous speech in these cases. This paper focuses on detecting some simple disfluencies in this speech type, using a Learning Cellular Automata. This is a set of cells that each is a learning automata (LA), and selects an action with a determined probability. Each LA is assigned to a word of the speech transcription sentence, and the action set for each LA includes four simple disfluencies types and "Fluent". The en
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Bailey, Dallin J., Michael Blomgren, Catharine DeLong, Kiera Berggren, and Julie L. Wambaugh. "Quantification and Systematic Characterization of Stuttering-Like Disfluencies in Acquired Apraxia of Speech." American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 26, no. 2S (2017): 641–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2017_ajslp-16-0108.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose The purpose of this article is to quantify and describe stuttering-like disfluencies in speakers with acquired apraxia of speech (AOS), utilizing the Lidcombe Behavioural Data Language (LBDL). Additional purposes include measuring test–retest reliability and examining the effect of speech sample type on disfluency rates. Method Two types of speech samples were elicited from 20 persons with AOS and aphasia: repetition of mono- and multisyllabic words from a protocol for assessing AOS (Duffy, 2013), and connected speech tasks (Nicholas & Brookshire, 1993). Sampling was repeated at 1
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Onslow, Mark, Kate Gardner, Kathryn M. Bryant, Cathi L. Stuckings, and Tamsin Knight. "Stuttered and Normal Speech Events in Early Childhood." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 35, no. 1 (1992): 79–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3501.79.

Full text
Abstract:
A set of 200 utterances from stuttering and normally speaking children aged 2–4 years was obtained. Each utterance contained a disfluency. A group of 5 sophisticated listeners assigned one of Johnson’s eight disfluency categories to each of the 200 utterances. These clinicians showed poor agreement in the categories they assigned. Subsequently, the 200 disfluencies were presented to a group of generalist clinician listeners and a group of unsophisticated listeners, who were asked to judge whether each disfluency was "stuttering" or "normal." The disfluencies judged with high agreement to be "s
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Shriberg, Elizabeth. "To ‘errrr’ is human: ecology and acoustics of speech disfluencies." Journal of the International Phonetic Association 31, no. 1 (2001): 153–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0025100301001128.

Full text
Abstract:
Unlike read or laboratory speech, spontaneous speech contains high rates of disfluencies (e.g. repetitions, repairs, filled pauses, false starts). This paper aims to promote ‘disfluency awareness’ especially in the field of phonetics –which has much to offer in the way of increasing our understanding of these phenomena. Two broad claims are made, based on analyses of disfluencies in different corpora of spontaneous American English speech. First, an Ecology Claim suggests that disfluencies are related to aspects of the speaking environments in which they arise. The claim is supported by eviden
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Whitney, Janet L., and Howard Goldstein. "Using Self-Monitoring to Reduce Disfluencies in Speakers with Mild Aphasia." Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54, no. 4 (1989): 576–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5404.576.

Full text
Abstract:
This study assessed the effects of treatment designed to teach mildly aphasic individuals to monitor disfluencies—revisions, repetitions, audible pauses—in their connected speech. Treatment effects were evaluated using a multiple baseline design across 3 subjects. All subjects showed immediate and dramatic reductions in the frequency of target behaviors in a picture description task when self-monitoring was introduced. Two subjects simultaneously reduced untargeted disfluencies, and all 3 generalized their improved speech to another discourse task. Treatment effects were unrelated to the accur
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Bakti, Mária, and Judit Bóna. "Self-monitoring processes in simultaneous interpreting." FORUM / Revue internationale d’interprétation et de traduction / International Journal of Interpretation and Translation 14, no. 2 (2016): 194–210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/forum.14.2.02bak.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract In psycholinguistics there is an agreement that self-monitoring is part of the speech production system, it serves the repair of speech errors and disfluencies occurring during the process of speech production. During simultaneous interpreting, where source language speech perception and target language speech production happen simultaneously, the analysis of self-monitoring is of particular importance. In our study we compare self-monitoring processes in the target language texts, interpreted from English into Hungarian, of professional interpreters and trainee interpreters. We exami
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Newman, Linda L., and Ann B. Smit. "Some Effects of Variations in Response Time Latency on Speech Rate, Interruptions, and Fluency in Children's Speech." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 32, no. 3 (1989): 635–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3203.635.

Full text
Abstract:
The present study was designed to examine adult-child interactions during conversation with respect to the effects of adult paralinguistic speech variations on the speech production of children. Four 4-year-old children served as subjects. A single-subject A-B-A design with counterbalancing and replication was implemented. Each subject participated in three 15-min conversations with an experimenter. The independent variable was the interspeaker pause time—the response time latency (RTL). During the 15-min conversations, the experimenter used either a 1-s or a 3-s RTL when responding to the chi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Byrd, Courtney T., Lisa M. Bedore, and Daniel Ramos. "The Disfluent Speech of Bilingual Spanish–English Children: Considerations for Differential Diagnosis of Stuttering." Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 46, no. 1 (2015): 30–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2014_lshss-14-0010.

Full text
Abstract:
PurposeThe primary purpose of this study was to describe the frequency and types of speech disfluencies that are produced by bilingual Spanish–English (SE) speaking children who do not stutter. The secondary purpose was to determine whether their disfluent speech is mediated by language dominance and/or language produced.MethodSpanish and English narratives (a retell and a tell in each language) were elicited and analyzed relative to the frequency and types of speech disfluencies produced. These data were compared with the monolingual English-speaking guidelines for differential diagnosis of s
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Schiel, Florian, and Christian Heinrich. "Disfluencies in the speech of intoxicated speakers." International Journal of Speech Language and the Law 22, no. 1 (2015): 19–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1558/ijsll.v22i1.24767.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Meddeb, Elizabeth. "Medium Transferability: Managing Speech-to-Text Disfluencies." International Journal of Technology, Knowledge, and Society 4, no. 2 (2008): 163–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.18848/1832-3669/cgp/v04i02/55847.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Naro, Anthony Julius, and Maria Marta Pereira Scherre. "Disfluencies in the analysis of speech data." Language Variation and Change 8, no. 1 (1996): 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0954394500001046.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACTIn our study of concord phenomena in spoken Brazilian Portuguese we found disfluencies, including apparent corrections, in about 15% of the relevant tokens in our corpus of recorded speech data. Disfluencies have very little effect on the rate of marking. When fluency is included in a variable rule analysis as a factor group containing categories for different types of disfluency, as well as for data without any disfluency, it is not selected as statistically significant. Furthermore, separate analyses of all data, only fluent data, and only disfluent data revealed no significant chang
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

De Nil, Luc F., Jayanthi Sasisekaran, Pascal H. H. M. Van Lieshout, and Paul Sandor. "Speech disfluencies in individuals with Tourette syndrome." Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58, no. 1 (2005): 97–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.06.002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Eggers, Kurt, and Sabine Van Eerdenbrugh. "Speech disfluencies in children with Down Syndrome." Journal of Communication Disorders 71 (January 2018): 72–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2017.11.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

MORIN-LESSARD, Elizabeth, and Krista BYERS-HEINLEIN. "Uh and euh signal novelty for monolinguals and bilinguals: evidence from children and adults." Journal of Child Language 46, no. 3 (2019): 522–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000918000612.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractPrevious research suggests that English monolingual children and adults can use speech disfluencies (e.g., uh) to predict that a speaker will name a novel object. To understand the origins of this ability, we tested 48 32-month-old children (monolingual English, monolingual French, bilingual English–French; Study 1) and 16 adults (bilingual English–French; Study 2). Our design leveraged the distinct realizations of English (uh) versus French (euh) disfluencies. In a preferential-looking paradigm, participants saw familiar–novel object pairs (e.g., doll–rel), labeled in either Fluent (“
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Rincon, Cristina, Kia Noelle Johnson, and Courtney Byrd. "An Introductory Examination of Speech Disfluencies in Spanish–English Bilingual Children Who Do and Do Not Stutter During Narratives." Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups 5, no. 1 (2020): 131–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2019_persp-19-00040.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose The purpose of this study is to examine the frequency and type of speech disfluencies (stuttering-like and nonstuttering-like) in bilingual Spanish–English (SE) children who stutter (CWS) to SE children who do not stutter (CWNS) during narrative samples elicited in Spanish and English to provide further diagnostic information for this population and preliminary data toward an expansion of this study. Method Participants included six bilingual SE children (three CWS, three CWNS) ranging in age from 5 years to 7;5 (years;months) and recruited from the surrounding Houston, Texas area. Par
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Hartsuiker, Robert J., and Lies Notebaert. "Lexical Access Problems Lead to Disfluencies in Speech." Experimental Psychology 57, no. 3 (2010): 169–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000021.

Full text
Abstract:
A picture naming experiment in Dutch tested whether disfluencies in speech can arise from difficulties in lexical access. Speakers described networks consisting of line drawings and paths connecting these drawings, and we manipulated picture name agreement. Consistent with our hypothesis, there were more pauses and more self-corrections in the low name agreement condition than the high name agreement condition, but there was no effect on repetitions. We also considered determiner frequency. There were more self-corrections and more repetitions when the picture name required the less frequent (
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

FOOK, Chong Yen, Hariharan MUTHUSAMY, Lim Sin CHEE, Abdul Hamid Bin ADOM, and Sazali Bin YAACOB. "Comparison of speech parameterization techniques for the classification of speech disfluencies." TURKISH JOURNAL OF ELECTRICAL ENGINEERING & COMPUTER SCIENCES 21 (2013): 1983–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.3906/elk-1112-84.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Zebrowski, Patricia M. "Duration of the Speech Disfluencies of Beginning Stutterers." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 34, no. 3 (1991): 483–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3403.183.

Full text
Abstract:
This study compared the duration of within-word disfluencies and the number of repeated units per instance of sound/syllable and whole-word repetitions of beginning stutterers to those produced by age- and sex-matched nonstuttering children. Subjects were 10 stuttering children [9 males and 1 female; mean age 4:1 (years:months); age range 3:2–5:0], and 10 nonstuttering children (9 males and 1 female; mean age 4:0; age range: 2:10–5:1). Mothers of the stuttering children reported that their children had been stuttering for 1 year or less. One 300-word conversational speech sample from each of t
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Dlouhá, O. "Disfluencies of speech in children with developmental dysphasia." Journal of Fluency Disorders 25, no. 3 (2000): 251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0094-730x(00)80331-9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Shriberg, Elizabeth E., Rebecca A. Bates, and Andreas Stolcke. "Integrated acoustic and language modeling of speech disfluencies." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 100, no. 4 (1996): 2848. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.416758.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Eggers, Kurt, Sabine Van Eerdenbrugh, and Courtney T. Byrd. "Speech disfluencies in bilingual Yiddish-Dutch speaking children." Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 34, no. 6 (2019): 576–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2019.1678670.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Brennan, Susan E., and Michael F. Schober. "How Listeners Compensate for Disfluencies in Spontaneous Speech." Journal of Memory and Language 44, no. 2 (2001): 274–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2753.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Begić, Leila, and Branka Babić. "INFLUENCE OF LENGTH OF SENTENCES ON THE FREQUENCY OF SPEECH DISFLUENCIES IN CHILDREN WHO STUTTER." Journal Human Research in Rehabilitation 7, no. 1 (2017): 50–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.21554/hrr.041706.

Full text
Abstract:
The main purpose of this study was to investigate whether the length of sentences has influence on the frequency of speech disfluencies for children who stutter. The participants included 30 children who stutter 19 male participants and 13 female participants, whose age ranged between 4 years and 8 months to 6 years and 11 months (56 to 83 months of age). Research was conducted in kindergartens and primary schools in Tuzla Canton in Bosnia and Herzegovina2 . The test consisted of 36 sentences. In relation to the length, sentences were divided into three groups: in the first group there were 9
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Winslow, Mary, and Barry Guitar. "The Effects of Structured Turn-Taking on Disfluencies." Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 25, no. 4 (1994): 251–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461.2504.251.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of structured conversational turn-taking on the amount and types of disfluencies as well as on the speech rate of a 5-year-old boy who stuttered. A single subject design with measures of disfluencies under conditions of structured turn-taking versus no turn-taking was used (ABAB withdrawal design). All analyses were performed on tape recordings of dinner-time conversations in the subject’s home. Results appear to indicate that disfluencies decreased when structured conversational turn-taking was instituted and increased when turn-taking cond
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Bakker, Klaas, and Gene J. Brutten. "Speech-Related Reaction Times of Stutterers and Nonstutterers." Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 55, no. 2 (1990): 295–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5502.295.

Full text
Abstract:
Speech-related reaction time measures (laryngeal premotor and adjustment time for /a/, labial initiation and physiological voice onset time for /pa/) and fluency-related measures (number of stutterings, number of normal disfluencies, and time needed to complete an oral reading) of 24 stutterers and a like number of nonstutterers were assessed to determine their diagnostic discriminative power. Discriminant analysis showed that stutterers were most effectively differentiated from normally fluent speakers by the total number of stutterings and normal disfluencies during oral reading and by the d
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Postma, Albert, and Herman Kolk. "The Effects of Noise Masking and Required Accuracy on Speech Errors, Disfluencies, and Self-Repairs." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 35, no. 3 (1992): 537–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3503.537.

Full text
Abstract:
The covert repair hypothesis views disfluencies as by-products of covert self-repairs applied to internal speech errors. To test this hypothesis we examined effects of noise masking and accuracy emphasis on speech error, disfluency, and self-repair rates. Noise reduced the numbers of disfluencies and self-repairs but did not affect speech error rates significantly. With accuracy emphasis, speech error rates decreased considerably, but disfluency and self-repair rates did not. With respect to these findings, it is argued that subjects monitor errors with less scrutiny under noise and when accur
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Chon, HeeCheong, Eric S. Jackson, Shelly Jo Kraft, Nicoline G. Ambrose, and Torrey M. Loucks. "Deficit or Difference? Effects of Altered Auditory Feedback on Speech Fluency and Kinematic Variability in Adults Who Stutter." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 64, no. 7 (2021): 2539–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2021_jslhr-20-00606.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose The purpose of this study was to test whether adults who stutter (AWS) display a different range of sensitivity to delayed auditory feedback (DAF). Two experiments were conducted to assess the fluency of AWS under long-latency DAF and to test the effect of short-latency DAF on speech kinematic variability in AWS. Method In Experiment 1, 15 AWS performed a conversational speaking task under nonaltered auditory feedback and 250-ms DAF. The rates of stuttering-like disfluencies, other disfluencies, and speech errors and articulation rate were compared. In Experiment 2, 13 AWS and 15 adult
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Yairi, Ehud, and Nicoline Ambrose. "A Longitudinal Study of Stuttering in Children." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 35, no. 4 (1992): 755–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3504.755.

Full text
Abstract:
The objectives of this pilot study were to establish methods for longitudinal research of stuttering in children and to provide preliminary data on the variations that occur in disfluencies during the developmental course of stuttering. Twenty-seven preschool-aged children were followed for a minimum of 2 years shortly after they began stuttering. Tape-recorded speech samples were obtained from the children at several intervals during this period. The number of various types of disfluencies was counted in the speech samples obtained in each testing period. Twenty-one children continued to be f
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Altiparmak, Ayse, and Gulmira Kuruoglu. "An analysis of speech disfluencies of Turkish speakers: Influence of educational background." Contemporary Educational Researches Journal 7, no. 3 (2017): 106–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.18844/cerj.v7i3.2652.

Full text
Abstract:
This research aims to analyze the influence of educational background on Turkish native speakers’ production rates of speech disfluencies. 56 native speakers of Turkish between ages 33-50 years and over 50-year-olds took part in the study. Each group was divided into two sub-groups according to the educational background of the participants. Prepared and unprepared speech samples of each participant were gathered via face-to-face interviews that were recorded and transcribed. As a result, participants with higher education levels produced more filled gaps than elementary/middle school graduate
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Goberman, Alexander M., and Michael Blomgren. "Parkinsonian speech disfluencies: effects of l-dopa-related fluctuations." Journal of Fluency Disorders 28, no. 1 (2003): 55–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0094-730x(03)00005-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

McRoberts, Gerald W., and Herbert H. Clark. "The role of lexical access in spontaneous speech disfluencies." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 100, no. 4 (1996): 2573. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.417416.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Bóna, Judit. "Clustering of disfluencies in typical, fast and cluttered speech." Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 33, no. 5 (2018): 393–405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2018.1513075.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Kidd, Celeste, Katherine S. White, and Richard N. Aslin. "Toddlers use speech disfluencies to predict speakers’ referential intentions." Developmental Science 14, no. 4 (2011): 925–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01049.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Corley, Martin, and Oliver W. Stewart. "Hesitation Disfluencies in Spontaneous Speech: The Meaning of um." Language and Linguistics Compass 2, no. 4 (2008): 589–602. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818x.2008.00068.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Lutz, Konnie C., and A. R. Mallard. "Disfluencies and rate of speech in young adult nonstutterers." Journal of Fluency Disorders 11, no. 4 (1986): 307–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730x(86)90018-5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Hall, Nancy E., Toyoko S. Yamashita, and Dorothy M. Aram. "Relationship Between Language and Fluency in Children With Developmental Language Disorders." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 36, no. 3 (1993): 568–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3603.568.

Full text
Abstract:
The present investigation addresses two primary hypotheses: (a) that a subset of children with developmental language disorders exhibits significantly more disfluencies than other children with language disorders and (b) that differences between the disfluent and nondisfluent groups observed in fluency may be related to differences in language deficits. Spontaneous language samples from 60 preschool children with developmental language disorders were analyzed for frequency and type of disfluencies. Comparisons of the frequency of disfluencies across subjects revealed that a subset of 10 subjec
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Hall, Nancy E. "Emergence of Stuttering-Like Disfluencies During Therapy: An Exploratory Study." Perspectives on School-Based Issues 12, no. 1 (2011): 18–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/sbi12.1.18.

Full text
Abstract:
Anecdotal reports exist on the emergence of stuttering-like behavior during the course of language therapy, yet there is little empirical evidence of its occurrence. The aim of the present study is to address this lack of evidence by surveying school-based speech-language pathologists on their experiences with this phenomenon. Results show nearly 50% of the 324 respondents report observing the appearance of disfluencies in the speech of previously fluent children during language therapy. Findings are discussed from the perspective of interacting processes of language formulation and speech pro
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Clark, Chagit E., Edward G. Conture, Tedra A. Walden, and Warren E. Lambert. "Speech-Language Dissociations, Distractibility, and Childhood Stuttering." American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 24, no. 3 (2015): 480–503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_ajslp-14-0198.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose This study investigated the relation among speech-language dissociations, attentional distractibility, and childhood stuttering. Method Participants were 82 preschool-age children who stutter (CWS) and 120 who do not stutter (CWNS). Correlation-based statistics (Bates, Appelbaum, Salcedo, Saygin, & Pizzamiglio, 2003) identified dissociations across 5 norm-based speech-language subtests. The Behavioral Style Questionnaire Distractibility subscale measured attentional distractibility. Analyses addressed (a) between-groups differences in the number of children exhibiting speech-langua
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Kubozono, Haruo. "Speech disfluencies and their modeling: Speech errors and stuttering in English and Japanese." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 100, no. 4 (1996): 2849. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.416759.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Kai, Atsuhiko, and Seiichi Nakagawa. "Comparison of continuous speech recognition systems with unknown-word processing for speech disfluencies." Systems and Computers in Japan 29, no. 9 (1998): 43–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1520-684x(199808)29:9<43::aid-scj5>3.0.co;2-j.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!