Academic literature on the topic 'Sunning behavior in animals'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Sunning behavior in animals.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Journal articles on the topic "Sunning behavior in animals"
Dow, Douglas D. "Dusting and Sunning by Australian Brush-turkeys." Emu - Austral Ornithology 88, no. 1 (March 1988): 47–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1071/mu9880047.
Full textKelley, Elizabeth A., Nina G. Jablonski, George Chaplin, Robert W. Sussman, and Jason M. Kamilar. "Behavioral thermoregulation inLemur catta: The significance of sunning and huddling behaviors." American Journal of Primatology 78, no. 7 (February 17, 2016): 745–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22538.
Full textHouston, David C. "A POSSIBLE FUNCTION OF SUNNING BEHAVIOR BY GRIFFON VULTURES, GYPS SPP., AND OTHER LARGE SOARING BIRDS." Ibis 122, no. 3 (April 3, 2008): 366–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919x.1980.tb00892.x.
Full textGannon, V. P. J., and D. M. Secoy. "Seasonal and daily activity patterns in a Canadian population of the prairie rattlesnake, Crotalus viridus viridis." Canadian Journal of Zoology 63, no. 1 (January 1, 1985): 86–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/z85-016.
Full textCristol, Daniel A., Jessica L. Armstrong, Justine M. Whitaker, and Mark H. Forsyth. "Feather-Degrading Bacteria do not Affect Feathers on Captive Birds." Auk 122, no. 1 (January 1, 2005): 222–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/auk/122.1.222.
Full textUnsöld, Markus, and Roland R. Melzer. "Sunning behaviour in ibis (Threskiornithidae) - Observations on four species and conclusions for captivity care." Der Zoologische Garten 79, no. 2-3 (January 2010): 89–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.zoolgart.2010.07.001.
Full textHart, Benjamin L. "Behavior of Sick Animals." Veterinary Clinics of North America: Food Animal Practice 3, no. 2 (July 1987): 383–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0749-0720(15)31159-2.
Full textŚWIĘCICKA, NATASZA. "Compulsive behavior in fur animals." Medycyna Weterynaryjna 74, no. 1 (2018): 5999–2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.21521/mw.5999.
Full textMoore, Janice. "The Behavior of Parasitized Animals." BioScience 45, no. 2 (February 1995): 89–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1312610.
Full textKustritz, Margaret V. Root. "Reproductive behavior of small animals." Theriogenology 64, no. 3 (August 2005): 734–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.theriogenology.2005.05.022.
Full textDissertations / Theses on the topic "Sunning behavior in animals"
Garner, Joseph P. "The aetiology of stereotypy in caged animals." Thesis, University of Oxford, 1999. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.670219.
Full textCulligan, Casey A. "Helping Animals, Helping Ourselves: Reciprocal Benefits of Prosocial Behaviors Directed Toward Animals." Antioch University / OhioLINK, 2019. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=antioch1572007635785939.
Full textYang, Eun-jin. "Social experience, hormones and aggressive behavior in the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis)." Access restricted to users with UT Austin EID, 2002. http://www.lib.umi.com/cr/utexas/fullcit?p3108539.
Full textSHETTEL-NEUBER, MARY JOYCE. "ZOO EXHIBIT DESIGN: A POST-OCCUPANCY EVALUATION AND COMPARISON OF ANIMAL ENCLOSURES." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1986. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/188163.
Full textBashaw, Meredith J. "Social behavior and communication in a herd of captive giraffe." Diss., Available online, Georgia Institute of Technology, 2004:, 2003. http://etd.gatech.edu/theses/available/etd-04082004-180020/unrestricted/bashaw%5Fmeredith%5Fj%5F200312%5Fphd.pdf.
Full textRodgers, Edmund W. "Sexual plasticity in a marine goby (Lythrypnus dalli) social, endocrine, and genetic influences on functional sex /." unrestricted, 2007. http://etd.gsu.edu/theses/available/etd-12022007-220715/.
Full textTitle from file title page. Mattew S Grober, committee chair; Kim Wallen, Charles Derby, Laura Carruth, Tim Bartness, committee members. Electronic text (107 p. : ill. (some col.)) : digital, PDF file. Description based on contents viewed Jan. 31, 2008. Includes bibliographical references. (p. 94-107)
Shelton, Delia S. "Environmental features influence complex behavior in small groups of animals." Thesis, Indiana University, 2016. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10161866.
Full textSimple environmental features can shape complex behavior. Identifying key aspects of the environment (e.g., temperature, structure, toxins) that lead to widespread consequences is of central importance in a changing world. The primary objective of my dissertation is to investigate how relatively simple aspects of the environment can influence small groups of animals in profound and complex ways. In the first three chapters, I report on experiments showing how small changes in the environment can affect the expression of behavior at different points in development and can have important physiological consequences for litters of mouse pups. I then report on two sets of experiments showing how subtle changes in the environment can dramatically affect spacing patterns and social dynamics of small groups of adult zebrafish. Together, my results emphasize the ways that subtle changes in the environment can have profound impacts on individuals and small groups. In both lines of work, I have found that a more accurate characterization of the phenomena, infant rodent development and zebrafish social behavior, requires the use of individual and group measures and that temperature, density, and pollutants can have a powerful effect on group responses. These results are important because they show that the physical environment can have profound effects on the phenotype, and that with a changing physical environment or anthropogenic change, dramatic differences may be observed in the behavior of groups.
Govindarajulu, Purnima T. "Constraints on kinship in predicting social behaviour in vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus)." Thesis, McGill University, 1993. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=55498.
Full textIn Chapter 2, effects of kinship on the distribution of aggression and support in feral vervet monkeys were investigated by comparing aggression and support between full sibs and maternal half sibs (within matrilines), and between paternal-half sibs and unrelated juveniles (between matrilines). The strong tendency to behave affiliatively to matrilineal members in Old World monkeys, and maternal control of offspring rank within matrilines, may constrain the ability of offspring to enhance inclusive fitness by behaving differentially to either paternal or maternal sibs based on their degree of relatedness.
Mills, Alexander Matthew Carleton University Dissertation Biology. "The influence of moonlight on the behavior of goatsuckers (Caprimulgidae)." Ottawa, 1985.
Find full textSharpe, Lynda L. "Play and social relationships in the meerkat (Suricata suricatta)." Thesis, Stellenbosch : Stellenbosch University, 2005. http://hdl.handle.net/10019.1/50419.
Full textENGLISH ABSTRACT: Despite more than three decades of research, and the postulation of more than 30 hypotheses of function, the adaptive significance of play remains unknown. This study quantitatively evaluated a selection of hypotheses of function, using data collected from a wild population of small, social carnivore, the meerkat, Suricata suricatta. The study found that although play in meerkats carried an energetic cost, with individuals modulating their frequency of play in response to their energy intake, none of the hypotheses evaluated by the study could identify the adaptive benefits that meerkats derived from play. Play did not increase 'social harmony' by reducing aggression between playmates, nor did it strengthen an individual's bonds to its social group, such that it remained in the group for longer, or contributed more to the group's cooperative activities. There was no evidence that meerkats used play to strengthen alliances between individuals, and young meerkats played no more frequently with their future dispersal partners than with matched controls with which they did not disperse. Play fighting experience did not improve a meerkat's subsequent fighting skills, and individuals that ultimately won the dominant breeding position within a group (through serious fighting) played no more frequently, and no more successfully, as youngsters, than the littermates that they defeated in combat. Although play was inhibited by aggression, meerkats did not use play to contest, assert or establish dominance status, and there was little evidence to suggest that the preference young meerkats showed for play partners that were well matched in age, size and ability arose from their use of play for self-assessment. This study assessed only those hypotheses of function that predicted benefits that were of importance to the inclusive fitness of the study species. For example, the enhancement of social harmony and group cohesion should be invaluable to a species whose survival is dependent upon social cooperation; and the high reproductive skew exhibited by this species places huge value upon fighting skill and the ability to win social dominance. As a consequence, this study's negative findings suggest strongly that play is not capable of providing these benefits, and that play behaviour is unlikely to be used for these purposes in any mammal species. I conclude that the most likely function of play (based on play's ubiquitous characteristics, and the findings of neurological research on rats) is the promotion of growth of the cerebral cortex.
AFRIKAANSE OPSOMMING: Ten spyte van meer as drie dekades van navorsing en die voorstelling van meer as 30 hipoteses oor funksie, bly die aanpassingswaarde van spelonbekend. Hierdie studie is 'n kwantitatiewe evaluasie van verskeie hipoteses oor funksie, en gebruik data versamel vanuit 'n wilde bevolking van 'n klein sosiale karnivoor, die meerkat, Suricata suricatta. Die studie het bevind dat hoewel spel in meerkaaie 'n energetiese koste beloop, met individue wat hul spelfrekwensie aanpas by energie-inname, geen-een van die hipoteses onder beskouing die aanpassingswaarde van spel vir meerkaaie kon verduidelik nie. Spel het nie "sosiale harmonie" bevorder deur die afuame in aggressie tussen speelmaats nie, en het ook nie 'n individu se verbintenis tot sy sosiale groep versterk sodat hy langer in die groep sou bly of meer sou bydra tot samewerkingsaktiwiteite nie. Daar was geen bewyse vir die gebruik van spel in die versterking van bondgenootskappe tussen individue nie, en jong meerkaaie het nie meer gereeld met toekomstige verspreidings-venote gespeel as met gepaarde kontroles saam met wie hulle nie uiteengegaan het nie. Speelse gevegte het nie 'n meerkat se daaropvolgende gevegsvermoëns verbeter nie, en die individue wat uiteindelik die dominante voortplantingsposisie in 'n groep gewen het (deur ernstige stryd) het nie meer gereeld ofmeer suksesvol as jongelinge gespeel in vergelyking met die werpselmaats wat hulle in die stryd oorwin het nie. Hoewel spel deur aggressie onderdruk is, het meerkaaie spel nie gebruik om dominante range te beveg, bevestig of tot stand te bring nie. Daar was min bewyse ter ondersteuning van die voorstel dat jong meerkaaie se voorkeur vir speelmaats wat hul gelyke is in ouderdom, grootte en vermoë, onstaan het in die gebruik van spel vir selfondersoek. Hierdie studie het slegs die hipoteses van funksie beskou wat voorspellings gemaak het oor die voordele wat belangrik is in die inklusiewe fiksheid van die studie-species. Byvoorbeeld, die verbetering van sosiale harmonie en groepsamehang behoort van onskatbare waarde te wees vir 'n species wat afhanklik is van sosiale samewerking vir oorlewing; en die hoë graad van voorkeuraanwas duidelik in hierdie species plaas groot waarde op gevegsvaardighede en die vermoë om sosiale dominansie te wen. Gevolglik dui hierdie ondersoek se negatiewe bevindinge daarop dat spel nie hierdie voordele kan bied nie, en dat speelgedrag heel waarskynlik nie vir hierdie doeleindes in enige soogdier-species gebruik word nie. Ek kom tot die gevolgtrekking dat die heel waarskynlikste funksie van spel (gebaseer op spel se alomteenwoordige kenmerke en die bevindinge van neurologiese navorsing op rotte) die bevordering van groei in die serebrale korteks is.
Books on the topic "Sunning behavior in animals"
The sunning behaviour of birds: A guide for ornithologists. Bristol: Bristol Ornithological Club, 1986.
Find full textNadine, Takvorian, ed. Busy animals: Learning about animals in autumn. Mankato, MN: Picture Window Books, 2011.
Find full textill, King Dave, Cradock-Watson Jane ill, and Hopkins Dave ill, eds. Night-time animals. New York: Aladdin Books, 1992.
Find full textBook chapters on the topic "Sunning behavior in animals"
Hafez, B., and E. S. E. Hafez. "Reproductive Behavior." In Reproduction in Farm Animals, 291–306. Baltimore, Maryland, USA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781119265306.ch19.
Full textArnold, John M. "Squid Mating Behavior." In Squid as Experimental Animals, 65–75. Boston, MA: Springer US, 1990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-2489-6_5.
Full textMizuno, Tooru M., Ashwini Padhi, Naomi Fineberg, Naomi A. Fineberg, Ashwini Padhi, Michael H. Bloch, James F. Leckman, et al. "Operant Behavior in Animals." In Encyclopedia of Psychopharmacology, 926–29. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-68706-1_281.
Full textBranch, Marc N. "Operant Behavior in Animals." In Encyclopedia of Psychopharmacology, 1–5. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-27772-6_281-2.
Full textBranch, Marc N. "Operant Behavior in Animals." In Encyclopedia of Psychopharmacology, 1164–67. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-36172-2_281.
Full textKnol, B. W. "Behavior." In Medical History and Physical Examination in Companion Animals, 272–85. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 1995. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-0459-3_22.
Full textTu, Xiaoyuan. "The Behavior System." In Artificial Animals for Computer Animation, 85–116. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/3-540-46593-6_7.
Full textVander Wall, Stephen B., and Kimberly G. Smith. "Cache-Protecting Behavior of Food-Hoarding Animals." In Foraging Behavior, 611–44. Boston, MA: Springer US, 1987. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-1839-2_22.
Full textMassen, Jorg J. M. "Friendships in Animals." In Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior, 1–6. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1899-1.
Full textde Vere, Amber J. "Personality in Animals." In Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior, 1–9. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_984-1.
Full textConference papers on the topic "Sunning behavior in animals"
Moriyama, Tohru. "Anticipatory behavior in animals." In COMPUTING ANTICIPATORY SYSTEMS. ASCE, 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1063/1.58244.
Full textChampion, Benjamin T., Matthew A. Joordens, and Blake M. Allan. "Tracking animals to determine swarm behavior." In 2015 10th System of Systems Engineering Conference (SoSE). IEEE, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/sysose.2015.7151952.
Full textDamiano, Luisa, and Hagen Lehmann. "Behavior coordination between animals, humans and robots." In 2014 Joint IEEE International Conferences on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL-Epirob). IEEE, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/devlrn.2014.6982947.
Full textLehmann, Hagen, Luisa Damiano, and Lorenzo Natale. "Workshop on Behavior Coordination between Animals, Humans and Robots." In HRI '15: ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction. New York, NY, USA: ACM, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/2701973.2714395.
Full textGavrilov, Maxim, and Vlada Prikhodchenko. "Development of New Behavior Patterns for Animals in Minecraft." In 2021 IEEE Conference of Russian Young Researchers in Electrical and Electronic Engineering (ElConRus). IEEE, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/elconrus51938.2021.9396218.
Full textPregowski, Piotr, Edyta Owadowska, and Jan Pietrzak. "Thermal imaging application for behavior study of chosen nocturnal animals." In Defense and Security, edited by Douglas D. Burleigh, K. Elliott Cramer, and G. Raymond Peacock. SPIE, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/12.546807.
Full textTsenkov, Yuriy, and V. Tsenev. "Continuous analysis of free-roaming animals' behavior with ear-tag device." In 2017 40th International Spring Seminar on Electronics Technology (ISSE). IEEE, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/isse.2017.8000993.
Full textMatteo Barbari, Leonardo Conti, and Stefano Simonini. "Spatial Identification of Animals in Different Breeding Systems to Monitor Behavior." In Livestock Environment VIII, 31 August - 4 September 2008, Iguassu Falls, Brazil. St. Joseph, MI: American Society of Agricultural and Biological Engineers, 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.13031/2013.25606.
Full textWang, Zheyuan, S. Abdollah Mirbozorgi, and Maysam Ghovanloo. "Towards a kinect-based behavior recognition and analysis system for small animals." In 2015 IEEE Biomedical Circuits and Systems Conference (BioCAS). IEEE, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/biocas.2015.7348456.
Full textSandikci, Selcuk, Pinar Duygulu Sahin, and A. Bulent Ozguler. "Vision based behavior analysis of laboratory animals: A 3D gradient based approach." In 2009 IEEE 17th Signal Processing and Communications Applications Conference (SIU). IEEE, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/siu.2009.5136335.
Full textReports on the topic "Sunning behavior in animals"
Zydlewski, Gayle Barbin, Garrett Staines, Haley Viehman, Haixue Shen, and Megan Altenritter. Interactions of aquatic animals with the ORPC OCGen® in Cobscook Bay, Maine: Monitoring behavior change and assessing the probability of encounter with a deployed MHK device. Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), October 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/1332311.
Full textTetzlaff, Sasha, Jinelle Sperry, Bruce Kingsburg, and Brett DeGregorio. Captive-rearing duration may be more important than environmental enrichment for enhancing turtle head-starting success. Engineer Research and Development Center (U.S.), September 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.21079/11681/41800.
Full textMengak, Michael T. Wildlife Translocation. U.S. Department of Agriculture, Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service, July 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.32747/2018.7210105.ws.
Full text