Academic literature on the topic 'Trail Making Test A e B'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Trail Making Test A e B.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Trail Making Test A e B"

1

Llinàs-Reglà, Jordi, Joan Vilalta-Franch, Secundino López-Pousa, Laia Calvó-Perxas, David Torrents Rodas, and Josep Garre-Olmo. "The Trail Making Test." Assessment 24, no. 2 (2016): 183–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191115602552.

Full text
Abstract:
The Trail Making Test (TMT) is used as an indicator of visual scanning, graphomotor speed, and executive function. The aim of this study was to examine the TMT relationships with several neuropsychological measures and to provide normative data in community-dwelling participants of 55 years and older. A population-based Spanish-speaking sample of 2,564 participants was used. The TMT, Symbol Digit Test, Stroop Color–Word Test, Digit Span Test, Verbal Fluency tests, and the MacQuarrie Test for Mechanical Ability tapping subtest were administered. Exploratory factor analyses and regression lineal models were used. Normative data for the TMT scores were obtained. A total of 1,923 participants (76.3%) participated, 52.4% were women, and the mean age was 66.5 years (Digit Span = 8.0). The Symbol Digit Test, MacQuarrie Test for Mechanical Ability tapping subtest, Stroop Color–Word Test, and Digit Span Test scores were associated in the performance of most TMT scores, but the contribution of each measure was different depending on the TMT score. Normative tables according to significant factors such as age, education level, and sex were created. Measures of visual scanning, graphomotor speed, and visuomotor processing speed were more related to the performance of the TMT-A score, while working memory and inhibition control were mainly associated with the TMT-B and derived TMT scores.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Rossini, Edward D., and Michael A. Karl. "The Trail Making Test A and B: A Technical Note on Structural Nonequivalence." Perceptual and Motor Skills 78, no. 2 (1994): 625–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1994.78.2.625.

Full text
Abstract:
The major structural aspect of the Trail Making Test, length of drawn lines needed to complete the tests, was measured and compared for the adult and school-aged child versions. Trail Making B is a markedly longer test than Trail Making A, 32% for adults and 27% for school-aged children. The interpretive assumptions that Trail Making B differs from Trail Making A only in terms of the cognitive skills needed to complete the test and the implicit interpretive bias toward minimizing the motor component of the tests were challenged in this technical note.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Tamez, Elaine, Joel Myerson, Lucy Morris, Desirée A. White, Carolyn Baum, and Lisa Tabor Connor. "Assessing Executive Abilities Following Acute Stroke with the Trail Making Test and Digit Span." Behavioural Neurology 24, no. 3 (2011): 177–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2011/139703.

Full text
Abstract:
The Trail Making Test and Digit Span are neuropsychological tests widely used to assess executive abilities following stroke. The Trails B and Digits Backward conditions of these tests are thought to be more sensitive to executive impairment related to frontal lobe dysfunction than the Trails A and Digits Forward conditions. Trails B and Digits Backward are also thought to be more sensitive to brain damage in general. Data from the Stroke and Lesion Registry maintained by the Washington University Cognitive Rehabilitation Research Group were analyzed to compare the effects of frontal versus nonfrontal strokes and to assess the effects of stroke severity. Results showed that the performance of patients with frontal and nonfrontal strokes was comparable in each condition of both the Trail Making Test and Digit Span, providing no support for the widely held belief that Trails B and Digits Backward are more sensitive to frontal lobe damage. Further, Trails A was as strongly correlated with stroke severity as Trails B, whereas Digits Backward was more strongly correlated with stroke severity than Digits Forward. Overall, the Trail Making Test and Digit Span are sensitive to brain damage but do not differentiate between patients with frontal versus nonfrontal stroke.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

McKeown, Kate, Emma Richards, Jessica Richardson, and Andrea Tales. "The Trails Making Test. Does a Single Trial Reflect Performance Capability?" OBM Neurobiology 05, no. 02 (2021): 1. http://dx.doi.org/10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2102100.

Full text
Abstract:
Information processing speed (Reaction time, RT) to a single administration of the Trails A and Trails B components of the Trail Making Test (TMT) is used in the assessment of brain and behavioural functional integrity across the lifespan in both clinical and research contexts. Although the clinical utility of such single trial-related and thus rapidly gained results, is recognised, it is possible that its administration as a single trial only, precludes its ability to provide a more in-depth and thus relevant representation of functional integrity per se, and it does not allow a range of ability to be examined. Because outcome from a single trial can be susceptible to the influence of spurious and extraneous effects we examined how, within a single testing session, RT varied with respect to the administration of four trials of both Trails A and B of the TMT, and how the effects may be associated with anxiety and self-consciousness. We examined how RT varied with respect to the administration of four trials of the Trail making test and compared the performance over each of these trials with that of the first trial. Between the third and fourth trial, questionnaires on anxiety and self-consciousness were administered. This paradigm was tested with fifty five younger adults (age range eighteen - thirty years). Our results indicated that repeating both Trails A and B of the TMT, administering the tests over four trials, revealed a significantly disproportionately slowed information processing speed (RT) to the first compared to consecutive trials, with the effect greatest for the more difficult or resource-demanding Trails B test. There were no significant correlations between change in information processing speed and anxiety or self-consciousness. The first of the four trials represents the only trial typically performed in the clinical application of this test. Our finding that the time to complete one single trial can be significantly slower compared to the response to additional trials, indicates that an individuals’ information processing speed can appear much slower than their actual ability. Such findings can be expected to be of particular relevance to the future use of this test clinically when an individual’s performance is measured and judged with respect to possible diagnosis, and in future research when group-level TMT performance is compared between younger and older adults for example.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Klusman, Lawrence E., Lloyd I. Cripe, and Carl B. Dodrill. "Analysis of Errors on the Trail Making Test." Perceptual and Motor Skills 68, no. 3_suppl (1989): 1199–204. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1989.68.3c.1199.

Full text
Abstract:
This study was designed to determine whether the number and/or types of errors on the Trail Making Test differentiated head-injured and normal control subjects. Errors on Part B were categorized into two types of shifting errors (from number to letter and from letter to number) and two types of sequencing errors (number and letter). Subjects consisted of 133 head-injured patients and 145 normal controls. Analysis showed that the frequency of errors on Parts A and B did not differ significantly between the two groups nor did the percentage of subjects making errors. Total shifting and sequencing errors also did not differentiate between the two groups. Although head-injured subjects were more likely than controls to err in shifting from letters to numbers, this finding was of no apparent clinical usefulness. The discriminative value of time scores was confirmed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Lee, Sol, Jung Ah Lee, and Hyun Choi. "Driving Trail Making Test part B: a variant of the TMT-B." Journal of Physical Therapy Science 28, no. 1 (2016): 148–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1589/jpts.28.148.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

OLIVERA-SOUZA, RICARDO DE, JORGE MOLL, LEIGH J. PASSMAN, et al. "Trail making and cognitive set-shifting." Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria 58, no. 3B (2000): 826–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x2000000500006.

Full text
Abstract:
We tested the hypothesis that Part B of the Trail Making Test (TMT) is a measure of cognitive set-shifting ability in 55 normal subjects with the conventional (written) TMT and a verbal adaptation, the "verbal TMT" (vTMT). The finding of a significant association between Parts B of TMT and vTMT (r = 0,59, p < 0,001), after correcting for age and education, supports the view that Part B of TMT is a valid measure of the ability to alternate between cognitive categories.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Fasoranti, Zarek, Alexander Enrique, Katie Hunzinger, Kelsey Bryk, Thomas Kaminski, and Thomas A. Buckley. "Concussion History Does Not Adversely Affect Trail Making Test Performance." Neurology 95, no. 20 Supplement 1 (2020): S12.2—S12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000719984.77986.de.

Full text
Abstract:
ObjectiveTo determine the effect of concussion history on the electronic version of the Trail Making Test (TMT) A and TMT-B performance.BackgroundThe effects of concussion history on cognitive performance when the individual is still young has received limited attention. There are inconsistent results in prior studies using computerized neurocognitive tests however the TMT assesses divergent neurologic systems including working memory, horizontal and vertical saccades, attention, cognitive processing, and executive function. While the TMT has routinely identified deficits acutely post-concussion, the long-term effect of prior concussions has not been established.Design/MethodsThis study utilized a cross-sectional design of 50 current intercollegiate division I athletes (56% female, age: 19.8 ± 1.3 y.o.). Participants completed a reliable concussion history questionnaire as well as the TMT-A and TMT-B on an iPad at baseline. The independent variable was concussion history and the dependent variables were total time on TMT-A and TMT-B which were compared by independent samples t-tests or Mann-Whitney U test.ResultsOf the 50 student athletes 26 (52%) reported at least one previously diagnosed concussion. There was no significant difference between groups for the TMT-A (No prior concussion: 22.9 + 6.1 sec and Concussion history: 19.7 + 4.1, U = 227.0, p = 0.10) or TMT-B (No prior concussion: 42.1 + 13.2 sec and concussion history: 38.1 + 11.3, t = 1.17, p = 0.25).ConclusionsThe results of this study suggest that while still in college, a history of one or more prior concussions, did not adversely affect neurocognitive performance on either the TMT-A or B test. As concussion history is sometimes associated with later life neurocognitive performance, future studies should assess TMT-A and B across the lifespan.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Corrigan, John D., and Nancy S. Hinkeldey. "Relationships between Parts A and B of the Trail Making Test." Journal of Clinical Psychology 43, no. 4 (1987): 402–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(198707)43:4<402::aid-jclp2270430411>3.0.co;2-e.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Närhi, Vesa, Pekka Räsänen, Riitta-Leena Metsäpelto, and Timo Ahonen. "Trail Making Test in Assessing Children with Reading Disabilities: A Test of Executive Functions or Content Information." Perceptual and Motor Skills 84, no. 3_suppl (1997): 1355–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1997.84.3c.1355.

Full text
Abstract:
The speed of performance on Part A, Part B, and on an experimental version containing alphabetical series (Part A Alphabetic) of the Trail Making Test was studied with 19 children with reading disabilities and 34 controls from Grades 4 to 6. When the test was used in discriminant profile fashion, children with reading disabilities showed a deficit compared with control children on Part B relative to Part A but did not relative to the new Part A Alphabetic. The results indicate that the performance of the children with reading disabilities on Part B is likely to be affected by their slowness on the alphabetical series. Based on these results we recommend that the speed of following the alphabetical series be assessed when using Part B of the Trail Making Test.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Trail Making Test A e B"

1

Renfrow, Stephanie Lei. "Trail Making Test Quotient (Trails B/ Trails A): A comparison with measures of executive functioning." NSUWorks, 2010. http://nsuworks.nova.edu/cps_stuetd/60.

Full text
Abstract:
This study examined the utility of the Trail Making Test Quotient (Trails B/ Trails A) in assessing executive functioning relative to that of common tests of executive function such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Category Test, and the Stroop Test. The purpose of the current study was to investigate the relationship of the Trail Making Test Quotient (Trails B/ Trails A) with other common tests of executive functioning (i.e., Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Stroop, Category Test) to determine whether these tests are measuring similar domains of functioning or whether Trail Making Test Quotient (Trails B/ Trails A) offers a more pure measure of executive functioning over and beyond that of Trail Making Test B alone or the difference score, Trail Making Test (Trails B- Trails A). A series of partial correlations were conducted involving the Trail Making Test scores (Quotient, Difference, and B [Raw]), and the scores of the executive functioning measures (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Category Test, and Stroop), controlling for age, education, and gender. Trails Quotient, Trails B Raw, and Trails Difference were found to significantly negatively correlate with WCST Total # of Categories. Only Trails B Raw and Trails Difference were found to significantly positively correlate with WCST Perseverative Responses and Category Error. None of the Trail Making Test measures used in this study were found to significantly correlate Stroop Interference. Correlation coefficients were compared to determine the strength of Trails Quotient's relationship with the aforementioned executive functioning measures relative to that of Trails Difference and Trails B Raw. Contrary to the hypotheses of the current study, the Trails Quotient demonstrated a significantly weaker correlation with WCST Total # of Categories, WCST Perseverative Responses, and Category Error than that of Trails Difference and Trails B Raw. Additionally, there were no significant differences in the correlation coefficients of Trails Quotient, Trails Difference, and Trails B Raw with Stroop Interference. However, upon further investigation using exploratory factor analyses, it was discovered that Trails Quotient may have represented a particular component of executive functioning more so than the Trails Difference and Trails B Raw. The results suggest that Trails Quotient offers a unique estimate of executive skill specific to cognitive organization, whereas Trails B Raw and Trails Difference represent multiple executive domains including regulatory and organizational abilities. Clinical practice will benefit from the current study's findings in that assessment of complex executive functioning will be more precise. Future research is needed to determine the utility of the Trails Quotient in identifying specific types and locations of brain injury. Assessment of specific impaired frontal skills common to degenerative dementias and traumatic brain injury may be possible with the use of Trails Quotient contingent upon further research. Future research into the domains of executive functioning and the Trail Making Test should focus on specific skills within regulatory and organizational components, and the development of normative data for Trails Quotient.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Santos, Juliano dos. "Validação do teste de trilhas - B (trail making test - B) para uso em pacientes brasileiros com câncer em cuidados paliativos." Universidade de São Paulo, 2011. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/7/7139/tde-18082011-074222/.

Full text
Abstract:
Introdução: Prejuízos na atenção, concentração e execução de tarefas, entre outras funções cognitivas, parecem ser frequentes em doentes com câncer e em cuidados paliativos e podem comprometer o cotidiano desses pacientes. No entanto, há poucos estudos sobre o tema e desconhecem-se testes validados para essa população no Brasil. Objetivo: Validar o Teste de Trilhas B (TT-B) para uso em pacientes com câncer em cuidados paliativos. Método: Trata-se de estudo metodológico, de validação de instrumento, que envolveu pacientes (n=94) em tratamento paliativo no Ambulatório de Quimioterapia do Instituto do Câncer do Estado de São Paulo e seus acompanhantes (n=39). Os avaliados realizaram o TT-B e responderam dados sócio-demográficos, sobre a presença e intensidade de dor, de fadiga, sobre a qualidade do sono, ansiedade e depressão, em dois momentos, com intervalo máximo de sete dias. Os doentes também foram caracterizados quanto à doença e tratamento. As validades convergente e divergente foram testadas pela correlação entre o desempenho no TT-B, mensurado pelo tempo para a realização do teste e o número de erros cometidos, com a intensidade da dor, da fadiga, da ansiedade, da depressão e do repouso atribuído ao sono da noite anterior às avaliações. A validade discriminante foi testada pela comparação do TT-B entre doentes e sadios. A estabilidade do TT-B, em doentes e sadios, foi avaliada por meio de teste e reteste. Resultados: Os doentes tinham câncer colo-retal (47,8%), câncer de mama (15,9%), a totalidade apresentava metástase, foram homens (52,1%), com idade média de 53 anos. Para os doentes, o tempo médio de realização do TT-B foi de 147,5 segundos na primeira e 132 segundos na segunda avaliação e o número de erros médio foi de 1 na primeira e 0,8 na segunda avaliação. Para os acompanhantes o tempo de realização do TT-B foi de 127,3 e 110,7 segundos na primeira e segunda avaliação respectivamente e o número de erros foi 0,9 na primeira e 0,6 na segunda avaliação. O instrumento discriminou doentes de sadios em relação ao tempo utilizado para a realização do teste na primeira (p=0,014) e na segunda (p=0,035) avaliação, indicando melhor desempenho para os sadios, mas não em relação ao número de erros. O teste foi estável entre os sadios nas duas avaliações, tanto em relação ao tempo (p=0,071) quanto em relação ao número de erros (p=0,352) e entre os doentes o instrumento foi estável apenas em relação ao número de erros (p=0,913). O TT-B não demonstrou correlações significativas com dor, fadiga, depressão, ansiedade e descanso, o que causou estranheza. Conclusão: O TT-B está em processo de validação. Mostrou-se capaz de discriminar doentes de sadios e foi estável entre os saudáveis. Recomenda-se a continuidade de estudos com amostras maiores e a utilização do TT-B em paralelo a outro instrumento que avalie a função executiva.<br>Introduction: Impairments in attention, concentration and execution of tasks, among other cognitive functions, seem to be frequent in patients with cancer in palliative care and they can affect patients daily life. However, there are few studies about the theme and to our knowledge there are no validated tests for this population in Brazil. Objective: To validate Trial Making TestB (TMT-B) for assessment of patients with cancer in palliative care. Methods: Methodological study to validate an instrument that involved patients (n=94) in palliative treatment at the Clinic of Chemotherapy of the Instituto do Câncer do Estado de São Paulo and their healthy accompanying person (n=39). Patients were tested on TMT-B and answered demographic data, questions about the presence of pain and intensity, fatigue, quality of sleep, anxiety and depression, in two moments, with maximum interval of seven days. The patients were also characterized according to disease and treatment. The convergent and divergent validities were tested by the correlation between the performance on TMT-B, measured by the amount of time required to complete the task and the number of mistakes, and pain intensity, fatigue, anxiety, depression and the rest attributed to sleep in the previous night before assessments. The discriminate validity was tested by the comparison of TMT-B between patients and healthy accompany persons. The stability of TMT-B, in patients and healthy companions was evaluated through test and re-test. Results: The patients had colorectal cancer (47.8%), followed by breast cancer (15.9%), the totality presented metastasis, most patients were men (52.1%), mean age was 53 years. For patients, mean time required to complete TMT-B was 147.5 seconds in the first and 132 seconds in the second assessments and the mean number of mistakes was 1 in the first and 0.8 in the second assessments. For the healthy accompany person, the time required to complete TMT-B was respectively 127.3 and 110.7 seconds in the first and second assessments and the number of mistakes was 0.9 in the first and 0.6 in the second assessments. The instrument discriminated patients from healthy accompany persons regarding time required to complete the test in the first (p=0.014) and second (p=0.035) assessments, indicating better performance for the healthy ones, but not in relation to the number of mistakes. The test was stable among healthy people in the two assessments regarding time (p=0.071) and number of mistakes (p=0.352); for patients, the instrument was stable only in relation to the number of mistakes (p=0.913). TMT-B did not demonstrate significant correlations with pain, fatigue, depression, anxiety and rest. Conclusion: TMT-B is in validation process. It was able to discriminate between patients and healthy accompany persons and was stable for the healthy ones. Further studies are recommended with larger samples as well as the use of TMT-B in parallel to other instrument that evaluates the executive function.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Siriyala, Kodhanda Karthik. "Determining Level of Cognitive Impairment via Computing Fractals using a Computer." Scholar Commons, 2018. https://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/7364.

Full text
Abstract:
Mild Cognitive Impairment is a condition that affects an individual's thinking and decision making capabilities. Specifically, it is one where an individual's capabilities of memorizing, thinking and decision making are less efficient when compared to others. In order to diagnose this condition, a conventional method is to provide the subject with a small challenge they should be completed using pen and paper. This thesis focuses on how this method can be converted to a computer based test. A data visualization tool named Processing has been used to develop a system that runs a game-like test, which is completed by individuals using a mouse. The system then saves the individual's mouse movements in the form of a CSV file. This files are used for further analyzed using JMP Pro on how this data can be used for determining cognitive abilities via computing a metric called Fractal, and what conclusions can be drawn. In order to achieve comparable results, readings from two diverse age groups have been collected. The results using a total of 12 subjects are convincing in that the tool can be used to as a marker for detecting cognitive impairment.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Patel, Kruti D. "Neuropsychological Factors Associated with Useful Field of View." Ohio University / OhioLINK, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=ohiou1395411154.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Matoso, Juliana Magalhães Duarte. "Desempenho cognitivo em idosos hipertensos e normotensos." Universidade do Estado do Rio de Janeiro, 2012. http://www.bdtd.uerj.br/tde_busca/arquivo.php?codArquivo=6174.

Full text
Abstract:
Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico<br>A população de idosos cresce rapidamente no Brasil. A prevalência de hipertensão arterial sistêmica (HAS) e distúrbios cognitivos é elevada nesta população. Testamos a hipótese de que a HAS reduz o desempenho cognitivos em idosos. Foram selecionamos idosos hipertensos e normotensos com idade &#8805; 60 < 80. O desempenho cognitivo foi avaliado pelo Cambridge Cognitive Examination Revised (CAMCOG-R), por subtestes do Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale v.3 (WAIS III), além do Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), e o dos Trail Making Tests A/B (TMT-A/B). O desempenho cognitivo avaliado pelo escore global do CAMCOG-R e do QI estimado do WAIS III está reduzido nos idosos hipertensos mesmo quando controlado pela escolaridade, depressão, estado geral de saúde e qualidade de vida. O desempenho cognitivo em diversos domínios específicos controlados para a escolaridade, depressão, estado geral de saúde e qualidade de vida, e avaliados pelo CAMCOG-R, WAIS III, TMT-A e RAVLT também está reduzido nos idosos hipertensos. O presente estudo sugere que a HAS está associada ao declínio do desempenho cognitivo global em idosos. Notadamente, o desempenho das funções executivas está reduzido nos idosos hipertensos. Especula-se que a HAS seja um fator de risco para o declínio progressivo do desempenho cognitivo e, portanto, para o desenvolvimento de demência.<br>The elderly population is growing rapidly in Brazil. The prevalence of hypertension and cognitive disorders is high in this population. We tested the hypothesis that hypertension is associated with decreased cognitive performance in the elderly. We selected normotensive and hypertensive subjects aged &#8805; 60 <80 years old. Cognition was assessed through Cambridge Cognitive Examination Revised (CAMCOG-R), subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale v.3 (WAIS III), the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), and the Trail Making Tests A/B (TMT-A/B). The cognitive performance assessed by the CAMCOG-R global score and the estimated WAIS III. QI is reduced in elderly hypertensives even when controlled for education, depression, health status and quality of life. The cognitive performance in diverse cognitive domains assessed through CAMCOG-R, WAIS III, TMT-A e RAVLT is also reduced in elderly hypertensives when contolled for education, depression, health status and quality of life. These results suggest that hypertension is associated with reduced global cognitive performance in eldely hypertensives. Notably, executive functions are reduced in eldely hypertensives. We speculate that hypertension might be a risk factor for the progressive decline in cognitive function and, therefore, for the development of dementia.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Riemer, Thomas G. [Verfasser]. "Der Berliner Shape Trail Test – Charakterisierung und Validierung eines neuen Trail Making Tests / Thomas G. Riemer." Berlin : Medizinische Fakultät Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, 2015. http://d-nb.info/1075757401/34.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Riemer, Thomas Günther [Verfasser]. "Der Berliner Shape Trail Test – Charakterisierung und Validierung eines neuen Trail Making Tests / Thomas G. Riemer." Berlin : Medizinische Fakultät Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, 2015. http://d-nb.info/1075757401/34.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Friberg, Marc. "Executive function, working memory and speech-in-noise recognition – Comparing a non-semantic black and white version of the Trail Making Test to the original Trail Making Test." Thesis, Linköpings universitet, Institutionen för datavetenskap, 2017. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva-141836.

Full text
Abstract:
In this thesis, the relationship between cognition and speech-in-noise recognition, in normally-hearing Swedish students, is examined. The Trail Making Test, hypothesized to measure a wide range of cognitive functions, including executive function and working memory, has been criticized for being a culturally biased measure, hence the need for a culturally unbiased version. A between-group experiment was conducted in which a non-semantically dependent version of the Trail Making Test was compared to the original Trail Making Test in order to test for psychometric equivalence. A total of 21 young normally-hearing Swedish students were given three tests: TMT or TMT (non-semantically dependent version), a Swedish Reading Span Task and a Swedish speech-in-noise recognition task. The B parts of the two Trail Making Test versions differed significantly and both were moderately to highly correlated to speech-in-noise and reading span performance. The results indicates that the original Trail Making Test is a more plausible index for executive function and strengthens the relationship between executive function, working memory and speech-in-noise recognition.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Andrews, Karen Anne Hope. "Normative indications for Xhosa-speaking unskilled workers on the trail making test and the stroop test." Thesis, Rhodes University, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/10962/d1002435.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of the study was to produce preliminary normative indications for the Trail Making Test and the Stroop Test, administered in English, on a non-clinical sample of black, Xhosa-speaking, unskilled individuals (N = 33), with an educational level of 11 – 12 years, in two age categories (18 –29 and 30 – 40 years). The sample was equally distributed for gender and level of education. Participants, who were required to have a basic proficiency in English, were from traditionally black township schools with relatively disadvantaged quality of education. Within-sample age and gender effects were investigated. There were no significant age effects on the Trail Making Test, whereas there was one significant difference between age groups on the Stroop Test with respect to the Color-Word task, and a result that strongly approached significance on the Word task, with the younger group performing better than the older group. There were no significant gender effects on the Trail Making Test, whereas there was one significant difference between genders on the Stroop Test with respect to the Word task, and a result that approached significance on the Color task, with females performing better than males. Normative indications for both measures were compared to available normative data on western populations with higher levels and more advantaged quality of education. This comparison revealed consistently poorer performances for both the Trail Making Test and the Stroop Test, confirming the need for localised normative datasets to facilitate accurate neuropsychological diagnoses on culturally disadvantaged individuals.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Bonnaud, Véronique. "L'évaluation cognitive et neuropsychologique des fonctions exécutives : le cas du trail making test." Poitiers, 2005. http://www.theses.fr/2005POIT5007.

Full text
Abstract:
La notion de "fonctions exécutives" est complexe et revêt de multiples dimensions. Pour mesurer de façon objective et reproductible ces fonctions, nous disposons d'outils dont le Trail Making Test (TMT) est un exemple. Afin d'appréhender les performances au TMT dans ses deux formes A et B, les cliniciens s'appuient sur les indices B-A ou B/A, supposés refléter le poids du processus d'alternance. L'objectif de notre étude est d'une part, de vérifier la validité de l'utilisation du TMT en pratique clinique à partir de l'indice B-A, et d'autre part, d'évaluer si le procesus d'alternance est le seul impliqué dans cette épreuve. Les résultats montrent que l'indice B-A ne permet pas d'évaluer de manière univoque le coût de l'alternance entre les deux lexiques impliqués et ceci pour trois raisons : les différences de longueur et d'organisation spatiale entre les parties A et B du TMT ont un effet sur les temps de réalisation, au détriment de la partie B. De plus, le type de lexique influence les performances, le lexique alphabétique étant plus long d'accès que le lexique numérique. Enfin la partie B du TMT apparaît multifactorielle : les performances à cette épreuve sont liées aux performances à différentes tâches évaluant des aspects distincts de l'administrateur central. Celui-ci semble jouer un rôle important dans la recherche en mémoire à long terme impliquée par l'accès au lexique alphabétique, alors que la procédure d'alternance semble pouvoir être réalisée sans l'intervention d'un contrôle exécutif<br>The concept of executive functions is complex and includes various aspects. We can use different tools to get an objective and reproductible evaluation of these functions, including the TMT. TMT can be assessed in the two forms, A and B, and psychologists used B-A or B/A indexes which represent the switching procedures. The first aim of our study is to examine the validity of TMT used in clinical practice with the B-A index, and the second objective is to assess if only the switching process is included in this task. Results showed the B-A index does not allow a non equivocal evaluation of the switching cost for three reasons : length and spatial organization between part A and B and lexicon types are sufficient to induce significant time differences in test performances, affecting more the part B. In addition the type of lexicons influences the performances. TMT part B performances are linked to task, exploring various components of the CE. Central Executive seems to have an important part in long term memory research associated with alphabetical lexicon access whereas switching operation could be performed without the involvement of an executive control
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Books on the topic "Trail Making Test A e B"

1

Reynolds, Cecil R. Comprehensive trail-making test: Examiner's manual. Pro-Ed, 2002.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Trail Making Test A e B"

1

Meyers, John E. "Trail Making Test." In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Springer International Publishing, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-57111-9_217.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

McKinlay, Audrey. "Trail Making Test." In Encyclopedia of Child Behavior and Development. Springer US, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-79061-9_2934.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Heller, Lois Jane, Celette Sugg Skinner, A. Janet Tomiyama, et al. "Trail-Making Test." In Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine. Springer New York, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1005-9_1538.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Meyers, John E. "Trail Making Test." In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Springer New York, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_217.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Bucks, Romola S. "Trail-Making Test." In Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine. Springer New York, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6439-6_1538-2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Bucks, Romola S. "Trail-Making Test." In Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine. Springer International Publishing, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-39903-0_1538.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Meyers, John E. "Trail Making Test." In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-56782-2_217-2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Lancia, Stefania, Jongkwan Choi, Jiyeong Baek, et al. "Trail Making Test Induces Prefrontal Cortex Activation as Revealed by a cw Wearable-Wireless fNIRS/DOT Imager." In Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology. Springer International Publishing, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-91287-5_22.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Plebani, Mario, Ada Aita, and Laura Sciacovelli. "Patient Safety in Laboratory Medicine." In Textbook of Patient Safety and Clinical Risk Management. Springer International Publishing, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-59403-9_24.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractLaboratory medicine in the healthcare system has recently been recognized as a fundamental service in the clinical decision-making process. Therefore, the notion of patient safety in laboratory medicine must be recognized as the assurance that harm to patients will be avoided, safe care outcomes will be enhanced through error prevention, and the total testing process (TTP) will be continuously improved.Although the goal for patient safety is zero errors, and although laboratory professionals have made numerous efforts to reduce errors in the last few decades, current research into laboratory-related diagnostic errors highlights that: (a) errors occur at every step of the TTP, mainly affecting phases at clinical interfaces; (b) despite the improvement strategies adopted, analytical quality remains a challenge; (c) errors are linked not only to clinical chemistry tests, but also to new, increasingly complex diagnostic testing.Medical laboratories must therefore implement effective quality assurance tools to identify and prevent errors in order to guarantee the reliability of laboratory information. Accreditation in compliance with the International Standard ISO 15189 represents the first step, establishing processes with excellence requirements and greater expectations of staff competency. Another important step in preventing errors and ensuring patient safety is the development of specific educational and training programs addressed to all professionals involved in the process, in which both technical and administrative skills are integrated. A wide variety of information is provided by a robust quality management system and consensus-approved Quality Indicators (QI) that identify undesirable events, evaluate the risk to the patient, and call for corrective and preventive actions. However, the effectiveness of the system depends on the careful analysis of data collected and on staff awareness of the importance of laboratory medicine to the healthcare process. The main task of the new generation of laboratory professionals should be to gain experience in “clinical laboratory stewardship.” In order to safeguard patients, laboratory professionals must assist clinicians in selecting the right test for the right patient at the right time and facilitate the interpretation of laboratory information.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

"Complex Trail Making Test." In Encyclopedia of Child Behavior and Development. Springer US, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-79061-9_6265.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Trail Making Test A e B"

1

Lunardini, Francesca, Matteo Luperto, Katia Daniele, et al. "Validity of digital Trail Making Test and Bells Test in elderlies." In 2019 IEEE EMBS International Conference on Biomedical & Health Informatics (BHI). IEEE, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/bhi.2019.8834513.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Cassidy, Bill, Gemma Stringer, and Moi Hoon Yap. "Mobile Framework for Cognitive Assessment: Trail Making Test and Reaction Time Test." In 2014 IEEE International Conference on Computer and Information Technology (CIT). IEEE, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/cit.2014.164.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Kovari, Attila, Jozsef Katona, Ilona Heldal, et al. "Examination of Gaze Fixations Recorded during the Trail Making Test." In 2019 10th IEEE International Conference on Cognitive Infocommunications (CogInfoCom). IEEE, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/coginfocom47531.2019.9089937.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Plotnik, Meir, Glen M. Doniger, Yotam Bahat, et al. "Immersive trail making: Construct validity of an ecological neuropsychological test." In 2017 International Conference on Virtual Rehabilitation (ICVR). IEEE, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icvr.2017.8007501.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Tonomura, Ryuta, Tadamitsu Tadamitsu, Atsushi Manji, Naoyuki Kubota, and Takenori Obo. "Rehabilitation Support System for Attentional Deficits Based on Trail-Making Test." In 2018 Joint 10th International Conference on Soft Computing and Intelligent Systems (SCIS) and 19th International Symposium on Advanced Intelligent Systems (ISIS). IEEE, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/scis-isis.2018.00177.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Prange, Alexander, Michael Barz, Anika Heimann-Steinert, and Daniel Sonntag. "Explainable Automatic Evaluation of the Trail Making Test for Dementia Screening." In CHI '21: CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. ACM, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3411764.3445046.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Mabuchi, Takuya, Naoyuki Kubota, Takahiro Takeda, Atsushi Manji, and Tadamitsu Matsuda. "A writing pressure analysis method for evaluation of trail making test using smart device." In 2017 6th International Conference on Informatics, Electronics and Vision & 2017 7th International Symposium in Computational Medical and Health Technology (ICIEV-ISCMHT). IEEE, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/iciev.2017.8338607.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Kan, Yoshio, and Koji Kashihara. "Evaluation of Temporal Change in Heart Rate Variability during the Advanced Trail Making Test." In 2016 5th IIAI International Congress on Advanced Applied Informatics (IIAI-AAI). IEEE, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/iiai-aai.2016.209.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Carminelli, Antonio, and Giuseppe Catania. "B-Spline Finite Element Formulation for Laminated Composite Shells." In ASME 2008 International Mechanical Engineering Congress and Exposition. ASMEDC, 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/imece2008-67420.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper presents a finite element formulation for the dynamical analysis of general double curvature laminated composite shell components, commonly used in many engineering applications. The Equivalent Single Layer theory (ESL) was successfully used to predict the dynamical response of composite laminate plates and shells. It is well known that the classic shell theory may not be effective to predict the deformational behavior with sufficient accuracy when dealing with composite shells. The effect of transverse shear deformation should be taken into account. In this paper a first order shear deformation ESL laminated shell model, adopting B-spline functions as approximation functions, is proposed and discussed. The geometry of the shell is described by means of the tensor product of B-spline functions. The displacement field is described by means of tensor product of B-spline shape functions with a different order and number of degrees of freedom with respect to the same formulation used in geometry description, resulting in a non-isoparametric formulation. A solution refinement method, making it possible to increase the order of the displacement shape functions without using the well known B-spline “degree elevation” algorithm, is also proposed. The locking effect was reduced by employing a low-order integration technique. To test the performance of the approach, the static solution of a single curvature shell and the eigensolutions of composite plates were obtained by numerical simulation and are then compared with known solutions. Discussion follows.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Hayashi, Takahiro, Charles C. Kim, Kohki Kumagai, Mitsuhiro Goto, and Shiro Otake. "Technical Basis for the Exemptions to Mandatory Post Weld Heat Treatment (PWHT) of SA-738 Grade B for Sec. III Div. 1 Subsection NE Application." In ASME 2017 Pressure Vessels and Piping Conference. American Society of Mechanical Engineers, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/pvp2017-65694.

Full text
Abstract:
With the current practice in steel making and welding process control, thick and large structural components fabricated from the widely used steel materials such as SA-738 Grade B have good material properties sufficient enough to meet the increasing demand of avoiding a large amount of Post Weld Heat Treatment (PWHT) applications at both shop and site. The original rule in the ASME Boiler and Pressure Vessel Code (the Code), however, allows the exemption to mandatory PWHT for SA-738 Grade B by limiting the applicable material thickness equal to or less than 1 3/4 in. (44 mm) for Section III, Division 1, Subsection NE application. A Code Case was proposed with the purpose to increase the maximum thickness for the exemption of mandatory PWHT from 1 3/4 in. (44 mm) in the original rule to 2 3/8 in. (60 mm). In order to demonstrate the applicability of the PWHT exemption for material with increased thickness, material tests have been performed mainly for the test data required in the ASME Sec. III Div. 1 Subsection NE for three different heats of the plates. The tests performed also include additional data to those required by the Code, such as fracture toughness (KJc) test, microstructural observation, hardness test and oblique Y-groove test for SMAW process. All the tests results for the base materials and weld joints have shown sufficient rationale to increase the exemption thickness to mandatory PWHT to 2 3/8 in. (60 mm).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!