Academic literature on the topic 'Trisyllabic'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Trisyllabic.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Trisyllabic"

1

Benadon, Fernando. "Metrical perception of trisyllabic speech rhythms." Psychological Research 78, no. 1 (February 16, 2013): 113–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0480-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Lahiri, Aditi, and Paula Fikkert. "Trisyllabic shortening in English: past and present." English Language and Linguistics 3, no. 2 (November 1999): 229–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1360674399000234.

Full text
Abstract:
Trisyllabic shortening or TSS is one of the most controversial processes in the history of English. Time after time, claims have been made about quantity variations, attributing them either to trisyllabic shortening, closed syllable shortening, or other mechanisms. Our examination of the nature of TSS in the history of English leads us to conclude that it differs from closed syllable shortening, which is syllable based, and that the preference for a maximal foot has remained the underlying incentive for maintaining vowel quantity variations throughout the centuries. However, the prosodic system has undergone dramatic changes and many features of TSS in the older and modern stages are not the same. Older TSS affected mostly inflected native words, while in Modern English, TSS causes alternations in derivationally related words with Romance suffixes. Interacting with open syllable lengthening, older TSS led to quantity alternations in inflectional paradigms which were later levelled out. Romance loans, both suffixed and nonsuffixed forms, were borrowed in their entirety and constrained by the prosodic structure of the language. Only later, when these words came to be derivationally related, were quantity alternations observable with TSS operating as a constraint dictated by the prosodic structure of the modern language. Thus, throughout the history of English, TSS has served the same purpose: it led to the preferred prosodic structure of the word.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Young-Kook Kwon and Ge-Soon Moon. "Notes on Trisyllabic Shortening in Early English." Korean Journal of Linguistics 39, no. 4 (December 2014): 701–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.18855/lisoko.2014.39.4.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Martínez-Paricio, Violeta, and Francesc Torres-Tamarit. "Trisyllabic hypocoristics in Spanish and layered feet." Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 37, no. 2 (May 21, 2018): 659–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11049-018-9413-4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Wang, Xiaomei, and Yen-Hwei Lin. "A unified approach to Tianjin trisyllabic tone sandhi: Metrical conditions and tonal complexity." Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America 2 (June 12, 2017): 19. http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/plsa.v2i0.4063.

Full text
Abstract:
Tianjin (Chinese) exhibits complex interactions among its disyllabic tone sandhi rules, leading to both left-to-right and right-to-left rule applications in trisyllabic sequences (cf. Chen 1986, X. Wang 2003). Which directionality to adopt for each particular trisyllabic sequence is arbitrary and cannot be accounted for by any known principles. Based on data from a recent acoustic study, our phonological analysis demonstrates that the seemingly ungoverned directionality is only apparent and that Tianjin tone sandhi rules apply only from left to right when both metrical and tonal complexity conditions are satisfied, thereby providing a unified account.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Murray, Robert W. "The Shortening of Stressed Long Vowels in old English." Diachronica 5, no. 1-2 (January 1, 1988): 73–107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/dia.5.1-2.05mur.

Full text
Abstract:
SUMMARY Although it is generally assumed that shortening of stressed vowels in Old English occurred in two environments (viz. preceding two consonants in trisyllabic words, e.g., samcucu and preceding three consonants in disyllabic words, e.g., broemblas, godspell ), it is argued in this paper that the weight of the two environments cannot be considered equal in terms of a syllabic phonology since the postulation of syllable structures is required which cannot be justified for pre-Old English; e.g., Vmb$lV and Vds$pV. It turns out that a re-examination of exemplary cases leaves the claim that shortening occurred in disyllabics preceding three consonants unsupported for two reasons. Firstly, a variety of evidence indicates the appropriateness of a trisyllabic reconstruction for sequences of the type VmblV (i.e., as Vm$bl$(1)V), thus allowing their inclusion with the other trisyllabic structures. Secondly, shortening in godspell can be viewed as a special case resulting from lexical contamination of original god with god. The phonological shortening of the trisyllablcs is seen as the first round in a series where shortening generalized from heavier to lighter environments. RÉSUMÉ Même s'il est généralement accepté que l'abrègement des voyelles toniques en ancien anglais se produit dans deux environnements différents (avant deux consonnes dans les trisyllabes, par exemple samcucu, et avant trois consonnes dans les disyllabes, par exemple broemblas, godspell), le présent exposé démontre que le poids de ces deux environnements ne peut être considéré égal en termes de phonologie syllabique puisqu'il présuppose la présence d'une structure syllabique qui ne peut être justifiée en ancien anglais (par exemple, Vmb$lV et Vds$pV). Une re-examination de ces cas démontre que l'abrègement des voyelles tonales dans les dissyllabes lorsqu'elles précèdent trois consonnes est une hypothèse sans fondement. Un nombre de phénomènes suggèrent que la reconstruction trisyllabique pour les séquences du genre VmblV (i.e., Vm$bl $(1)V) est appropriée et nous permet de les inclure parmis les autres structures trissyllabiques, alors que l'abrègement de godspell peut être perçu comme un cas spécial résultant de la contamination lexicale entre god et god. L'abrègement dans les formes trisyllabiques est donc perçu comme étant la première étape d'une série de développements, où les abrègements se sont généralisés à partir des environnements syllabiques 'lourds', jusqu'aux environnements syllabiques 'légers'. ZUSAMMENFASSUNG Es wird gewöhnlich angenommen, daß die Verkürzung der betonten Vokale im Altenglischen in zwei Umgebungen stattfand; nämlich in drei-silbigen Wôrtern vor zwei Konsonanten (z.B. samcucu) und in zweisilbigen Wörtern vor drei Konsonanten (z.B. broemblas, godspell ). In diesem Aufsatz wird gezeigt, daB das Gewicht dieser zwei Umgebungen in einer Silbenphonologie nicht gleichgestellt werden können, da Silbenstrukturen (z.B. Vmb$lV und Vds$pV) erforderlich wären, die für das Altenglische nicht gerechtfertigt werden können. Tatsächlich liefert eine Nachprüfung der Muster-beispiele keine Unterstützung für die Annahme einer frühen Verkürzung der Vokallänge in zweisilbigen Wörtern vor drei Konsonanten. Im Fall einer Folge VmblV hingegen rechtfertigt eine Reihe von Tatsachen eine dreisilbige Rekonstruktion, d.h. Vm$bl $(1)V; folglich können diese Strukturen den anderen dreisilbigen zugeordnet werden. AuBerdem muß die Verkürzung in godspell als eine Art lexikalische Kontamination von god mit god betrachtet werden. Die Verkürzung dieser dreisilbigen Wôrter wird als Anfang eines Ent-wicklungsgangs betrachtet, wobei die Verkürzung zuerst schwere Umgebungen und dann immer leichtere Umgebungen betrifft.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Potisuk, Siripong, Jack Gandour, and Mary P. Harper. "Contextual Variations in Trisyllabic Sequences of Thai Tones." Phonetica 54, no. 1 (1997): 22–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000262208.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Chung, Chin-Wan. "A Foot-Based Approach to Trisyllabic Shortening in English." Journal of Mirae English Language and Literature 24, no. 4 (November 30, 2019): 99–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.46449/mjell.2019.11.24.4.99.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Wee, Lian Hee. "Casual speech elision and tone sandhi in Tianjin trisyllabic sequences." International Journal of Chinese Linguistics 1, no. 1 (September 5, 2014): 71–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/ijchl.1.1.03wee.

Full text
Abstract:
In the Tianjin dialect, casual utterance of familiar trisyllabic sequences often induces deletion of phonological segments so that for a trisyllabic string, the non-final syllables would merge into a single syllable. This elide-and-merge process interacts with the rich Tianjin tone sandhi system to produce rather complicated patterns. In this paper, casual speech elision is shown to fall out straightforwardly from a model that recognizes morae as associated with segments and also as tone-bearing units. Thus, elision of morae also removes tonal features. While this understanding provides a clear description of the patterns, it also reveals an ordering paradox: sandhi applies before elision in some cases, but after elision in others. The paradox is resolved by favoring the order that produces a contour tone for the merged syllable. An explanation for this can be found if one recognizes that Tianjin is prosodically iambic.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

ARCIULI, Joanne, Lucia COLOMBO, and Luca SURIAN. "Lexical stress contrastivity in Italian children with autism spectrum disorders: an exploratory acoustic study." Journal of Child Language 47, no. 4 (December 12, 2019): 870–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000919000795.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractWe investigated production of lexical stress in children with and without autism spectrum disorders (ASD), all monolingual Italian speakers. The mean age of the 16 autistic children was 5.73 years and the mean age of the 16 typically developing children was 4.65 years. Picture-naming targets were five trisyllabic words that began with a weak–strong pattern of lexical stress across the initial two syllables (WS: matita) and five trisyllabic words beginning with a strong–weak pattern (SW: gomito). Acoustic measures of the duration, fundamental frequency, and intensity of the first two vowels for correct word productions were used to calculate a normalised Pairwise Variability Index (PVI) for WS and SW words. Results of acoustic analyses indicated no statistically significant group differences in PVIs. Results should be interpreted in line with the exploratory nature of this study. We hope this study will encourage additional cross-linguistic studies of prosody in children's speech production.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Trisyllabic"

1

Maye, Jessica. "Trisyllabic Shortening and Two Affix Classes." University of Arizona Linguistics Circle, 2000. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/311825.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Kim, Misty Noelani. "Psychometrically Equivalent Trisyllabic Words for Speech Reception Threshold Testing in Cantonese." Diss., CLICK HERE for online access, 2007. http://contentdm.lib.byu.edu/ETD/image/etd1824.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Keller, Laurel Anne. "Psychometrically Equivalent Trisyllabic Words for Speech Reception Threshold Testing in Spanish." Diss., CLICK HERE for online access, 2009. http://contentdm.lib.byu.edu/ETD/image/etd2776.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Graham, Jessica Lee. "Psychometrically Equivalent Trisyllabic Words for Testing Spanish Pediatric Speech Recognition Thresholds." BYU ScholarsArchive, 2016. https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd/5864.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this study was to use previously recorded Spanish trisyllabic words tested on adults in the measurement of the speech recognition threshold of Spanish-speaking children in order to (a) determine the words' appropriateness when testing children and (b) compare psychometric functions between adults and children. A selection of 28 frequently used trisyllabic words was chosen from previously recorded samples of male and female adult native speakers of Spanish. These words were then presented to 20 native Spanish-speaking children with normal hearing between the ages of 4 and 8 years. The words were presented starting at -5 dB HL and ascended in 5 dB increments until the presentation level reached 15 dB HL. Using logistic regression, psychometric functions were calculated for each word. Resulting pediatric thresholds were found to be 8.7 dB higher for male talkers and 11.0 dB higher for female talkers than previously reported adult thresholds. These results indicate a clinically significant threshold difference between pediatric and adult populations. Future research should be conducted to measure the speech recognition threshold (SRT) in children of varying ages to determine the age at which the SRT approximates with adult performance.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Jennings, Lara-Jill. "Psychometrically Equivalent Digital Recordings for Speech Audiometry Testing in Mandarin Chinese: Standard Mandarin Dialect." Diss., CLICK HERE for online access, 2005. http://contentdm.lib.byu.edu/ETD/image/etd1124.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Mangum, Tanya Crawford. "Performance Intensity Functions for Digitally Recorded Japanese Speech Audiometry Materials." Diss., CLICK HERE for online access, 2005. http://contentdm.lib.byu.edu/ETD/image/etd838.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Trisyllabic"

1

Carignan, Christopher, and Zsuzsanna Fagyal. "V-to-V assimilation in trisyllabic words in French." In Romance Linguistics 2009, 25–42. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/cilt.315.02car.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Luo, Jianfei. "A Structural and Prosodic Analysis of Trisyllabic New Words." In Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 120–27. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-49508-8_12.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Trisyllabic"

1

Türk, Helen, Pärtel Lippus, Karl Pajusalu, and Pire Teras. "Temporal patterns of geminates in Inari Saami trisyllabic words." In 10th International Conference on Speech Prosody 2020. ISCA: ISCA, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/speechprosody.2020-17.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Xiong, Ziyu, Qiguang Lin, Maolin Wang, and Zhouyu Chen. "The effect of focus on trisyllabic syllable duration in Mandarin." In 2019 22nd Conference of the Oriental COCOSDA International Committee for the Co-ordination and Standardisation of Speech Databases and Assessment Techniques (O-COCOSDA). IEEE, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/o-cocosda46868.2019.9041173.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Veroňková, Jitka, and Giorgi Gersamia. "Vowel length of Czech trisyllabic words in L1 Russian speakers." In 11th International Conference of Experimental Linguistics. ExLing Society, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.36505/exling-2020/11/0052/000467.

Full text
Abstract:
Languages differ in how vowel length is applied, which can affect the acquisition of a second language. In Czech, the length is phonological. There are practically no restrictions on its occurrence. It is also completely independent of word stress. In Russian, the length of vocals does not have a phonological status but can be an accompanying characteristic in the implementation of a word stress. The subject of the experiment is the perceptual analysis of vocal quantity in Czech as L2 in Russian speakers (8 subjects). The material consists of recordings of a set of trisyllabic words (48 lexemes, 256 items), in which the structure of quantity (8 different patterns) is checked.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Tu, Jung-Yueh, Janice Wing-Sze Wong, and Jih-Ho Cha. "Trisyllabic Tone 3 Sandhi Patterns in Mandarin Produced by Cantonese Speakers." In Interspeech 2017. ISCA: ISCA, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/interspeech.2017-291.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Lai, Wei, and Jianjing Kuang. "Prosodic grouping in Chinese trisyllabic structures by multiple cues—tone coarticulation, tone sandhi and consonant lenition." In Tonal Aspects of Languages 2016. ISCA, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/tal.2016-34.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography