Academic literature on the topic 'Upward and downward social comparison subscales'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Upward and downward social comparison subscales.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Upward and downward social comparison subscales"

1

Ozimek, Phillip, Hans-Werner Bierhoff, and Elke Rohmann. "How Downward and Upward Comparisons on Facebook Influence Grandiose and Vulnerable Narcissists’ Self-Esteem—A Priming Study." Behavioral Sciences 11, no. 3 (2021): 39. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/bs11030039.

Full text
Abstract:
Past research showed that social networking sites represent perfect platforms to satisfy narcissistic needs. The present study aimed to investigate how grandiose (GN) and vulnerable narcissism (VN) as well as social comparisons are associated with Facebook activity, which was measured with a self-report on three activity dimensions: Acting, Impressing, and Watching. In addition, the state self-esteem (SSE) was measured with respect to performance, social behavior, and appearance. One hundred and ten participants completed an online survey containing measures of SSE and Facebook activity and a
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Michinov, Nicolas, and Louis Bavent. "Upward Persistence and Downward Desistence: Some Reactions to Social-Comparison Deprivation after a Threat." European Psychologist 6, no. 2 (2001): 112–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027//1016-9040.6.2.112.

Full text
Abstract:
Laboratory and field research has shown that people prefer either downward or upward comparison after a threatening experience. Downward comparison is generally used to protect self-esteem immediately after a threat. It can be regarded as a short-term self-protection strategy. Upward comparison is often used to see whether it is possible to improve a situation by finding similarities with more fortunate people. It can be regarded as a long-term self-improvement strategy. It was assumed here that deprivation of downward and upward comparison after a threat would generate different degrees of in
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Michinov, Nicolas, and Jean-Marc Monteil. "UPWARD OR DOWNWARD COMPARISON AFTER FAILURE. THE ROLE OF DIAGNOSTIC INFORMATION." Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal 25, no. 4 (1997): 389–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.1997.25.4.389.

Full text
Abstract:
What leads people to select an upward or a downward social comparison after a threat? This experiment examines a basic interpretation proposed in the literature according to which, in a threatening situation, the choice of direction for a social comparison, upward or downward, depends on the self-improvement or self-enhancement goals pursued by the subjects. The results provide empirical support for this interpretation and show that diagnostic information about the target can change the direction of social comparison. Indeed, when the diagnostic information allows subjects to hold out some hop
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Foley, Sharon, Hang-yue Ngo, and Raymond Loi. "Antecedents and consequences of upward and downward social comparisons." International Journal of Organizational Analysis 24, no. 1 (2016): 145–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/ijoa-02-2014-0743.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to extend and test a theory of uncertainty and directional social comparisons. Prior studies have posited that uncertainty leads to increased upward and downward social comparisons. The authors ' view is that uncertainty affects upward and downward comparisons differentially. They test their theory in the Chinese workplace, and focus specifically on employees’ comparisons of career progress. Workplace consequences of social comparisons are also investigated. Design/methodology/approach – The authors achieve their objectives by collecting data from respond
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Gibbons, Frederick X., and Meg Gerrard. "Effects of Upward and Downward Social Comparison on Mood States." Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 8, no. 1 (1989): 14–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1989.8.1.14.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Taylor, Shelley E., and Marci Lobel. "Social comparison activity under threat: Downward evaluation and upward contacts." Psychological Review 96, no. 4 (1989): 569–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.96.4.569.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Lee, Hannah S., and David A. Griffith. "Social Comparison in Retailer–Supplier Relationships: Referent Discrepancy Effects." Journal of Marketing 83, no. 2 (2019): 120–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022242918823542.

Full text
Abstract:
Social comparisons among suppliers connected through a common retailer pose significant management challenges for the retailer. For instance, a focal supplier’s social comparison can result in upward or downward referent discrepancy, decreasing or increasing perceptions of distributive fairness, respectively, subject to the tie strength of the relationship. Because decreasing perceptions of distributive fairness can be harmful to the retailer–supplier relationship, the authors examine the use of tie strength and timing of explanations as actions a retailer can take to mitigate such perceptions
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Michinov, Nicolas. "WHEN DOWNWARD COMPARISON PRODUCES NEGATIVE AFFECT: THE SENSE OF CONTROL AS A MODERATOR." Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal 29, no. 5 (2001): 427–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2001.29.5.427.

Full text
Abstract:
It has been assumed that engaging in upward or downward comparison can either improve or deteriorate affect, depending on the amount of control individuals feel they have over the comparison dimension. The main goal of the present study was to determine whether an individual difference factor such as the sense of control can moderate the relationship between social comparison and affect. The results showed that for downward comparison, the lower the participants scored on the sense of control, the higher the negative affect they experienced. Unexpectedly, the sense of control was unrelated to
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Sherrard, Carol A. "Elderly Wellbeing and the Psychology of Social Comparison." Ageing and Society 14, no. 3 (1994): 341–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0144686x00001628.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACTTwenty-two elderly retired people were interviewed for their beliefs about the sources of wellbeing in old age. Manual and Professional social class responses were compared, controlling for age, gender and health status. Respondents' free discourse was characterised by spontaneous social comparisons of the self with other people. In Social Comparison Theory, these serve as a means of self-assessment or wellbeing-enhance-ment. The comparison statements were analysed by Direction, Target, Dimension, and Wellbeing Yield. Significant class differences were apparent. Both groups compared Do
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Tiggemann, Marika, and Janet Polivy. "Upward and Downward: Social Comparison Processing of Thin Idealized Media Images." Psychology of Women Quarterly 34, no. 3 (2010): 356–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2010.01581.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!