To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Vocalisations.

Journal articles on the topic 'Vocalisations'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Vocalisations.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Webster, Patrick, Nicholas Leseberg, Stephen Murphy, and James Watson. "Descriptions of the vocalisations of the Painted Button-quail Turnix varius in North Queensland." Australian Field Ornithology 40 (2023): 111–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.20938/afo40111119.

Full text
Abstract:
The Painted Button-quail Turnix varius is a widespread and frequently encountered ground-dwelling bird endemic to Australia. Despite being regularly recorded throughout its distribution, our understanding of this species’ vocalisations is poor. Published accounts are limited to brief descriptions without quantitative analyses in a standardised format. This presents challenges in separating the vocalisations of this species from other similar species, particularly as vocalising button-quail are seldom observed. We recorded the vocalisations of Painted Button-quail from North Queensland. These v
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Stafford, Kathleen M., Sharon L. Nieukirk, and Christopher G. Cox. "Geographic and seasonal variation of blue whale calls in the North Pacific." J. Cetacean Res. Manage. 3, no. 1 (2023): 65–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.47536/jcrm.v3i1.902.

Full text
Abstract:
The call characteristics and distribution of blue whales in the North Pacific were examined by use of acoustic surveys. Two distinct vocalisation types have been previously attributed to blue whales from limited regions in the North Pacific (cf. Thompson and Friedl, 1982; Rivers, 1997). Hydrophone data from sixteen sites in the North Pacific were examined for these blue whale vocalisations. There were distinct geographic and seasonal differences between the occurrence of the two vocalisation types. The hydrophones that were more westerly recorded the ‘northwestern’ Pacific vocalisation, those
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Corkeron, Peter J., and Sofie M. Van Parijs. "Vocalizations of eastern Australian Risso's dolphins, Grampus griseus." Canadian Journal of Zoology 79, no. 1 (2001): 160–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/z00-180.

Full text
Abstract:
Recordings were made from a group of Risso's dolphins (Grampus griseus) off the coast of Newcastle, Australia, between 30 Hz and 22 kHz. Risso's dolphins exhibited seven separate vocalisation types: broadband clicks, barks, buzzes, grunts, chirps, whistles, and simultaneous whistle + burst-pulse sounds. Broadband clicks were highly variable in duration, with a frequency range of 6 to >22 kHz. Bark vocalisations consisted of highly variable burst pulses, with durations ranging from 0.2 to 7.4 s and a frequency range of 2-20 kHz. Buzz vocalisations were clearly stereotyped, consisting of a sh
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Torquet, Nicolas, Fabrice de Chaumont, Philippe Faure, Thomas Bourgeron, and Elodie Ey. "mouseTube – a database to collaboratively unravel mouse ultrasonic communication." F1000Research 5 (September 16, 2016): 2332. http://dx.doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.9439.1.

Full text
Abstract:
Ultrasonic vocalisation is a broadly used proxy to evaluate social communication in mouse models of neuropsychiatric disorders. The efficacy and robustness of testing these models suffer from limited knowledge of the structure and functions of these vocalisations as well as of the way to analyse the data. We created mouseTube, an open database with a web interface, to facilitate sharing and comparison of ultrasonic vocalisations data and metadata attached to a recording file. Metadata describe 1) the acquisition procedure, e.g., hardware, software, sampling frequency, bit depth; 2) the biologi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

von Borell, E., B. Bünger, T. Schmidt, and T. Horn. "Vocal-type classification as a tool to identify stress in piglets under on-farm conditions." Animal Welfare 18, no. 4 (2009): 407–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0962728600000816.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractPrevious studies have shown that the analysis of high frequency stress calls in pigs can serve as a reliable tool in welfare research. Our study focuses on the classification of three different classes of piglet vocalisation: grunting, squealing and screaming. In a castration experiment (Experiment 1), 3,285 vocalisations from 42 piglets were analysed for 21 different vocal characteristics. A first discriminant function for the three vocal types was derived from recordings made under laboratory-like conditions. A second discriminant function was derived from non-calibrated measurements
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Orihuela, A., and R. Ungerfeld. "Acoustic characteristics of vocalisations emitted by the domestic rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) during copula ejaculation and electro-ejaculation with or without anaesthesia." World Rabbit Science 27, no. 3 (2019): 157. http://dx.doi.org/10.4995/wrs.2019.10809.

Full text
Abstract:
Vocalisations can be used as reliable indicators of pain, but little information is available in rabbits, where acoustic tools for farming environments can be used for welfare judgements. The aim of this study was to compare vocalisations produced during copula ejaculation and electro-ejaculation (EE), with or without general anaesthesia, in domestic rabbits. Vocalisations of nine New Zealand white adult males were digitally recorded. The number of males vocalising and vocal characteristics including high, low, maximum and fundamental frequencies and duration of the vocalisations were analysed
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Rankin, Shannon, Don Ljungblad, Chris Clark, and Hidehiro Kato. "Vocalisations of Antarctic blue whales, Balaenoptera musculus intermedia, recorded during the 2001/2002 and 2002/2003 IWC/SOWER circumpolar cruises, Area V, Antarctica." J. Cetacean Res. Manage. 7, no. 1 (2023): 13–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.47536/jcrm.v7i1.752.

Full text
Abstract:
Blue whale vocalisations recorded during the 2001/2002 and 2002/2003 International Whaling Commission-Southern Ocean Whale and Ecosystem Research (IWC/SOWER) cruises were analysed to determine the feasibility of using acoustic recordings for sub-species identification of the Antarctic blue whale (Balaenoptera musculus intermedia) and the pygmy blue whale (B.m. brevicauda). The research was conducted in IWC Area V, from latitude 60°S to the ice edge and between longitudes 130°E and 150°E on the Shonan Maru (2001/2002), and between 150°E and 170°W on the Shonan Maru No.2 (2002/2003). Data includ
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Webster, Patrick, Nigel Jackett, George Swann, Nicholas Leseberg, Stephen Murphy, and James Watson. "Descriptions of the vocalisations of the Chestnut-backed Button-quail Turnix castanotus." Australian Field Ornithology 38 (2021): 137–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.20938/afo38137144.

Full text
Abstract:
Button-quail (Turnicidae) are a greatly understudied family of birds; their cryptic habits make studying them in the wild challenging. They are known to be quite vocal which could assist with detectability, and so it follows that survey results will be more reliable as knowledge about button-quail vocal repertoire increases. Until recently, there were no published vocal recordings of the Chestnut-backed Button-quail Turnix castanotus, and descriptions and accounts of this species’ vocalisations were limited. We recorded vocalisations of Chestnut-backed Button-quail at six locations across the
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Azhari, Atiqah, Paola Rigo, Marc H. Bornstein, and Gianluca Esposito. "Where Sounds Occur Matters: Context Effects Influence Processing of Salient Vocalisations." Brain Sciences 10, no. 7 (2020): 429. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10070429.

Full text
Abstract:
The social context in which a salient human vocalisation is heard shapes the affective information it conveys. However, few studies have investigated how visual contextual cues lead to differential processing of such vocalisations. The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is implicated in processing of contextual information and evaluation of saliency of vocalisations. Using functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS), we investigated PFC responses of young adults (N = 18) to emotive infant and adult vocalisations while they passively viewed the scenes of two categories of environmental contexts: a domes
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Du, Xiaodong, Lenn Carpentier, Guanghui Teng, Mulin Liu, Chaoyuan Wang, and Tomas Norton. "Assessment of Laying Hens’ Thermal Comfort Using Sound Technology." Sensors 20, no. 2 (2020): 473. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/s20020473.

Full text
Abstract:
Heat stress is one of the most important environmental stressors facing poultry production and welfare worldwide. The detrimental effects of heat stress on poultry range from reduced growth and egg production to impaired health. Animal vocalisations are associated with different animal responses and can be used as useful indicators of the state of animal welfare. It is already known that specific chicken vocalisations such as alarm, squawk, and gakel calls are correlated with stressful events, and therefore, could be used as stress indicators in poultry monitoring systems. In this study, we fo
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Cato, Douglas, and Tracey Rogers. "INDIVIDUAL VARIATION IN THE ACOUSTIC BEHAVIOUR OF THE ADULT MALE LEOPARD SEAL, HYDRURGA LEPTONYX." Behaviour 139, no. 10 (2002): 1267–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853902321104154.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractVocalisations and vocalising bouts of adult male leopard seals recorded from the Prydz Bay region, East Antarctica, were compared to determine whether they showed individual variability. There were distinct individual patterns in the sequence of vocalisations within vocalising bouts. A sequence could be reliably ascribed to a particular individual with a high degree (83%) of certainty. Such sequences may carry information about the identity of the caller. Acoustic characteristics of the two most commonly used vocalisations, the high and low double trills, showed weak within individual
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Thiebault, Andréa, Isabelle Charrier, Thierry Aubin, David B. Green, and Pierre A. Pistorius. "First evidence of underwater vocalisations in hunting penguins." PeerJ 7 (December 18, 2019): e8240. http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.8240.

Full text
Abstract:
Seabirds are highly vocal on land where acoustic communication plays a crucial role in reproduction. Yet, seabirds spend most of their life at sea. They have developed a number of morphological, physiological and behavioural adaptations to forage in the marine environment. The use of acoustic signals at sea could potentially enhance seabirds’ foraging success, but remains largely unexplored. Penguins emit vocalisations from the sea surface when commuting, a behaviour possibly associated with group formation at sea. Still, they are unique in their exceptional diving abilities and feed entirely
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Best, Paul, Sébastien Paris, Hervé Glotin, and Ricard Marxer. "Deep audio embeddings for vocalisation clustering." PLOS ONE 18, no. 7 (2023): e0283396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0283396.

Full text
Abstract:
The study of non-human animals’ communication systems generally relies on the transcription of vocal sequences using a finite set of discrete units. This set is referred to as a vocal repertoire, which is specific to a species or a sub-group of a species. When conducted by human experts, the formal description of vocal repertoires can be laborious and/or biased. This motivates computerised assistance for this procedure, for which machine learning algorithms represent a good opportunity. Unsupervised clustering algorithms are suited for grouping close points together, provided a relevant repres
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Viscardi, AV, and PV Turner. "Use of meloxicam, buprenorphine, and Maxilene® to assess a multimodal approach for piglet pain management, part 2: tail-docking." Animal Welfare 28, no. 4 (2019): 499–510. http://dx.doi.org/10.7120/09627286.28.4.499.

Full text
Abstract:
Piglets on commercial pig farms are often tail-docked to reduce the incidence of tail-biting. While this is a painful procedure, piglets are often not provided analgesia or anaesthesia for pain relief. The objectives of this study were to assess a multimodal approach to managing tail-docking pain in piglets, using 0.4 mg kg–1 meloxicam (MEL), 0.04 mg kg–1 buprenorphine (BUP), and Maxilene® (MAX), a topical anaesthetic. The effectiveness of each drug and drug combination was evaluated using behavioural indicators, vocalisation, and facial grimace analysis. This study also assessed whether male
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Muller, Benjamin J., David A. Pike, and Lin Schwarzkopf. "Defining the active space of cane toad (Rhinella marina) advertisement calls: males respond from further than females." Behaviour 153, no. 15 (2016): 1951–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-00003404.

Full text
Abstract:
Many animals produce advertisement vocalisations to attract mates. A vocalisation’s active space is the area within which a receiver responds to it, while its maximum extent occurs when a receiver stops responding. We mapped behavioural responses of male and female cane toads (Rhinella marina) to advertisement calls, by conducting experimental playbacks to: (i) examine attenuation of a cane toad call, (ii) define the active space of these vocalisations, by measuring phonotaxis at different distances from the call, and (iii) quantify the active space of calls for both sexes, separately. The cal
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Muzzi, Gerson M., Marina H. L. Duarte, Nathália O. Braga, Rogério P. Bastos, and Luciana B. Nascimento. "Acoustic monitoring of anuran communities in road noise disturbed soundscapes." Herpetological Journal 33, no. 2 (2023): 34–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.33256/33.2.3442.

Full text
Abstract:
Vocalisation is the main form of communication in frogs. These signals have different social structures and functions. Road noise has increased over the past few decades to the point where it can mask acoustic signals with impacts for animal communication. Anurans are sensitive to noise masking, but few studies have investigated how noise impacts their vocal behaviour. Here, we compared noise levels and activity, as well as calling activity and richness of aurally-identified species, between two sites; one near and one far from a road. We also assessed the potential of noise masking. Noise was
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Boisseau, O., D. Gillespie, R. Leaper, and A. Moscrop. "Blue (Balaenoptera musculus) and fin (B. physalus) whale vocalisations measured from northern latitudes of the Atlantic Ocean." J. Cetacean Res. Manage. 10, no. 1 (2023): 23–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.47536/jcrm.v10i1.656.

Full text
Abstract:
Vocalisations were recorded in the vicinity of sighted blue whales (Balaenoptera musculus) and fin whales (B. physalus) in the North Atlantic between Iceland and Greenland in August 2004 from a hydrophone towed behind a research vessel and from free floating sonobuoys. The structures of recorded calls were broadly similar to those reported from other areas, but lacked the stereotypical patterning of those signals thought to represent reproductive displays. Counts of non-patterned blue whale calls indicated low vocalisation rates, with a mean of 0.62 phrases per whale per hour (0.12 A-B and 0.4
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Aloufi, Aliaa. "Onomatopoeia and Cat Vocalisations." International Journal of English Linguistics 11, no. 5 (2021): 14. http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ijel.v11n5p14.

Full text
Abstract:
Onomatopoeia—the imitation of natural sounds—is a common phenomenon in human language, though imitations of the same sounds might appear different cross-linguistically. It is true that onomatopoeia is not like ordinary language, but how does it differ from natural vocalisation? While the distinction between onomatopoeia and ordinary language has received ample treatment, its difference from natural sounds have so far received less attention from linguistics. This study aims to investigate the phonetic differences between onomatopoeic cat sounds in ten languages and natural
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Dempster, E. R. "Vocalisations of adult Northern Quolls, Dasyurus hallucatus." Australian Mammalogy 17, no. 1 (1994): 43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1071/am94005.

Full text
Abstract:
Vocalisations of Northern Quolls, Dasyurus hallucatus, were recorded during staged interactions between pairs of the same sex and different sexes. Four types of vocalisations were identified: 'hisses', 'sniffs', 'squawks' and 'twitters'. No pure ultrasonic calls were identified, although frequencies of twitters and sniffs extended into the ultrasonic range. All vocalisations were broadband, noisy sounds, with little evidence of harmonic structure or frequency modulation. Vocalisations of D. hallucatus are compared with dat
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Yang, L., X. Xu, and P. Berggren. "Spotted seal Phoca largha underwater vocalisations in relation to ambient noise." Marine Ecology Progress Series 683 (February 3, 2022): 209–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.3354/meps13951.

Full text
Abstract:
Animals use different strategies to adjust their vocalisations to compensate for ambient noise interference. This is true for some marine mammals, especially cetaceans, but relatively little is known about this for pinnipeds. We recorded 4 major call types (drum, growl, knock, and sweep) of spotted seals Phoca largha in Liaodong Bay, China, to investigate if seals adjusted their vocalisation parameters in relation to broadband (50-4000 Hz) ambient noise recorded immediately preceding each seal vocalisation. Regression analyses showed that the received level of growls, in both broadband and 1/3
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Morfi, Veronica, Yves Bas, Hanna Pamuła, Hervé Glotin, and Dan Stowell. "NIPS4Bplus: a richly annotated birdsong audio dataset." PeerJ Computer Science 5 (October 7, 2019): e223. http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj-cs.223.

Full text
Abstract:
Recent advances in birdsong detection and classification have approached a limit due to the lack of fully annotated recordings. In this paper, we present NIPS4Bplus, the first richly annotated birdsong audio dataset, that is comprised of recordings containing bird vocalisations along with their active species tags plus the temporal annotations acquired for them. Statistical information about the recordings, their species specific tags and their temporal annotations are presented along with example uses. NIPS4Bplus could be used in various ecoacoustic tasks, such as training models for bird pop
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Silva, Chamath K., Vinuri Piyathilake, Chamath Keppitiyagama, et al. "A Wavelet Transform-based Feature Extraction Pipeline for Elephant Rumble Detection." International Journal on Advances in ICT for Emerging Regions (ICTer) 16, no. 3 (2023): 21–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.4038/icter.v16i3.7268.

Full text
Abstract:
Elephants generate infrasonic vocalisations that traverse through the air for long distances. Utilising this phenomenon, a previous work proposed a system, called Eloc, to localise and track elephants in the wild. The Eloc system has been demonstrated to be accurate in calculating the location of infrasonic sources. However, it still lacks the capability to accurately distinguish elephant infrasonic calls from various other infrasonic sources using limited computing power on board. Addressing this problem, the work presented in this paper introduces an approach to distinguish elephant infrason
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Pozo Galván, Yaiza Pilar, María Pérez Tadeo, Morgane Pommier, and Joanne O’Brien. "Static Acoustic Monitoring of Harbour (Phoca vitulina) and Grey Seals (Halichoerus grypus) in the Malin Sea: A Revolutionary Approach in Pinniped Conservation." Journal of Marine Science and Engineering 12, no. 1 (2024): 118. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/jmse12010118.

Full text
Abstract:
Harbour and grey seals rely on acoustic signals to mate, socialise and defend their territory. Previous studies have focused on their behaviour, movements and communication from the coast, leaving a knowledge gap in the offshore environments, and therefore being unable to determine the proper use they give to those areas and the risks they face around them. Acoustic data collected with a SoundTrap were analysed to assess the detectability of both species in the Malin Sea. Vocalisations were classified based on aural and visual features, as well as using non-parametric classification trees. Dif
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Pahl, B. C., J. M. Terhune, and H. R. Burton. "Repertoire and Geographic Variation in Underwater Vocalisations of Weddell Seals (Leptonychotes weddellii, Pinnipedia : Phocidae) at the Vestfold Hills, Antarctica." Australian Journal of Zoology 45, no. 2 (1997): 171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1071/zo95044.

Full text
Abstract:
The underwater vocalisations of Weddell seals (Leptonychotes weddellii) near Davis, Antarctica, were many and varied. A total of 11029 vocalisations recorded throughout and immediately after the breeding season were analysed. Vocalisations were classified by cluster analysis techniques, based on differences in frequency, duration, call shape, waveform and number of elements. Thirteen broad call categories (many with subdivisions) were identified. Twelve call types (belonging to nine categories) made up 91· 9% of the vocalisations and were present at all seven study sites within the Vestfold Hi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Lisboa, Carolina Almeida, Francisco Dyonísio Cardoso Mendes, Maurício Silveira, and Ludmilla Moura de Souza Aguiar. "The Vocal Repertoire of the Bearded Capuchin (Cebidae: Sapajus libidinosus): Implications for Understanding the Complexity of Neotropical Primate Communication." Folia Primatologica 92, no. 3 (2021): 151–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000517917.

Full text
Abstract:
Vocal communication is an essential aspect of primate social behaviour. The bearded capuchin <i>Sapajus libidinosus</i> is endemic to Brazil, and some studies have described specific vocalisation types for this species; however, there is still no complete description of its vocal repertoire. Thus, this study aimed to describe the vocal repertoire of a group of <i>S. libidinosus</i> living in the<i></i>Parque Nacional de Brasília, a protected area in the Cerrado area of Central Brazil. We carried out focal samplings and recording of vocalisations of members o
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Schön, PC, B. Puppe, and G. Manteuffel. "Automated recording of stress vocalisations as a tool to document impaired welfare in pigs." Animal Welfare 13, no. 2 (2004): 105–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s096272860002683x.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe vocalisations of animals are results of particular emotional states. For example, the stress screams of pigs may be indicators of disturbed welfare. Our objective was to develop a system to monitor and record levels of stress calls in pigs, which could be employed in environments of breeding, transportation and slaughter. Using a combination of sound analysis by linear prediction coding and artificial neural networks, it was possible to detect the stress vocalisations of pigs in noisy pig units with few recognition errors (<5%). The system (STREMODO: stress monitor and documenta
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Balsby, Thorsten, Simon Boel Pedersen, and Torben Dabelsteen. "Degradation of whitethroat vocalisations: implications for song flight and communication network activities." Behaviour 140, no. 6 (2003): 695–719. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853903322370634.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractTransmission of acoustic signals through the habitat modifies the signals and may thus influence their use in communication. We investigated the transmission of five different types of whitethroat (Sylvia communis) vocalisations, three types of song and two calls. Typical examples were broadcast and re-recorded in a whitethroat habitat with hedgerows and open meadow. We used a complete factorial design with speaker and microphone placed in different natural sender and receiver positions including high perches and song flights. Sound degradation was quantified in terms of signal-to-nois
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Pahl, BC, JM Terhune, and HR Burton. "Proportional Weekly Use of Underwater Call Types by Weddell Seals, Leptonychotes Weddellii (Pinnipedia: Phocidae), During the Breeding Season at the Vestfold Hills." Australian Journal of Zoology 44, no. 1 (1996): 75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1071/zo9960075.

Full text
Abstract:
Underwater vocalisations by Weddell seals, Leptonychotes weddellii, were recorded during the 1992 breeding season, at the Vestfold Hills, Antarctica. Only 3 of the 12 major call types recorded at all sites had statistically significant variations in utilisation throughout the season. No consistent trends were evident. The underwater calls of Weddell seals increased;in number during the breeding season, hut the proportional weekly usage of each of the major call types did not change. The absence of vocalisation changes suggests that the breeding behaviours of Weddell seals are not synchronous a
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Lötters, Stefan, Dietrich Mebs, Gunther Köhler, Joseph Vargas, and Enrique La Marca. "The voice from the hereafter: vocalisations in three species of Atelopus from the Venezuelan Andes, likely to be extinct." Herpetozoa 32 (December 4, 2019): 267–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.3897/herpetozoa.32.e39192.

Full text
Abstract:
Atelopus is a species-rich group of Neotropical bufonids. Present knowledge on bioacoustics in this genus is relatively poor, as vocalisations have been described in only about one fifth of the ca. 100 species known. All studied members of the genus produce vocalisations although, with a few exceptions, most species lack a middle ear. Nonetheless, hearing has been demonstrated even in earless Atelopus making bioacoustics in these toads an inspiring research field. So far, three structural call types have been identified in the genus. As sympatry is uncommon in Atelopus, calls of the same type
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Moody, Carly M., Georgia J. Mason, Cate E. Dewey, and Lee Niel. "Getting a grip: cats respond negatively to scruffing and clips." Veterinary Record 186, no. 12 (2019): 385. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/vr.105261.

Full text
Abstract:
Use of scruffing and scruffing tools (eg, clipnosis clips) to immobilise cats is contentious, and cat handling guidelines vary in recommendations regarding these techniques. The current study examined whether cats show negative responses to the following restraint methods: (1) scruff (n=17), (2) clip application to the dorsal neck skin (n=16) and (3) full body (a known negative; n=19). Each cat was also handled with passive restraint (control) for comparison. During handling, cats were examined for behavioural (side/back ear positions, vocalisations, lip licking) and physiological (pupil dilat
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Lötters, Stefan, Dietrich Mebs, Gunther Köhler, Joseph Vargas, and Marca Enrique La. "The voice from the hereafter: vocalisations in three species of Atelopus from the Venezuelan Andes, likely to be extinct." Herpetozoa 32, no. () (2019): 267–75. https://doi.org/10.3897/herpetozoa.32.e39192.

Full text
Abstract:
Atelopus is a species-rich group of Neotropical bufonids. Present knowledge on bioacoustics in this genus is relatively poor, as vocalisations have been described in only about one fifth of the ca. 100 species known. All studied members of the genus produce vocalisations although, with a few exceptions, most species lack a middle ear. Nonetheless, hearing has been demonstrated even in earless Atelopus making bioacoustics in these toads an inspiring research field. So far, three structural call types have been identified in the genus. As sympatry is uncommon in Atelopus, calls of the same type
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Austin, Victoria I., Anastasia H. Dalziell, Naomi E. Langmore, and Justin A. Welbergen. "Avian vocalisations: the female perspective." Biological Reviews 96, no. 4 (2021): 1484–503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/brv.12713.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Fahn, S. "Paroxysmal myoclonic dystonia with vocalisations." Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 50, no. 1 (1987): 117–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.50.1.117.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Powys, Vicki. "Weebill Vocalisations—An Undescribed Trill." Emu - Austral Ornithology 99, no. 4 (1999): 295–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1071/mu99034c.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Newson, Pauline. "Caring strategies for inappropriate vocalisations." Nursing and Residential Care 14, no. 10 (2012): 539–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.12968/nrec.2012.14.10.539.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Garson, P. J., Elizabeth Shillito Walser, S. J. G. Hall, and Margaret A. Vince. "Vocalisations of the Chillingham Cattle." Behaviour 104, no. 1-2 (1988): 78–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853988x00610.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Wu, Shih-Hung, Jerome Chie-Jen Ko, Ruey-Shing Lin, Wen-Ling Tsai, and Hsueh-Wen Chang. "An acoustic detection dataset of birds (Aves) in montane forests using a deep learning approach." Biodiversity Data Journal 11 (February 24, 2023): e97811. https://doi.org/10.3897/BDJ.11.e97811.

Full text
Abstract:
Long-term monitoring is needed to understand the statuses and trends of wildlife communities in montane forests, such as those in Yushan National Park (YSNP), Taiwan. Integrating passive acoustic monitoring (PAM) with an automated sound identifier, a long-term biodiversity monitoring project containing six PAM stations, was launched in YSNP in January 2020 and is currently ongoing. SILIC, an automated wildlife sound identification model, was used to extract sounds and species information from the recordings collected. Animal vocal activity can reflect their breeding status, behaviour, populati
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Pérez Tadeo, María, Martin Gammell, and Joanne O'Brien. "First Steps towards the Automated Detection of Underwater Vocalisations of Grey Seals (Halichoerus grypus) in the Blasket Islands, Southwest Ireland." Journal of Marine Science and Engineering 11, no. 2 (2023): 351. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/jmse11020351.

Full text
Abstract:
Underwater vocalisations of grey seals (Halichoerus grypus) were recorded by static acoustic monitoring (SM2M, Wildlife Acoustics) in the vicinity of a colony located at White Strand beach on Great Blasket Island, southwest Ireland during the pre-breeding and breeding seasons. Grey seal vocalisations were first classified across nine different categories based on aural and visual characteristics of the spectrograms, providing an acoustic repertoire for grey seals. This classification was further investigated by applying a classification tree analysis, resulting in five of the initial nine grou
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Landers, Todd J., Carol A. Bannock, and Mark E. Hauber. "Dynamics of behavioural rhythms in a colonial, nocturnal, burrowing seabird: a comparison across different temporal scales." Notornis 58, no. 2 (2011): 81. https://doi.org/10.63172//300940lnzmer.

Full text
Abstract:
There have been few studies on the temporal patterns of social behaviours and how they relate to timing of life history stages in nocturnal colonial bird species. This study focuses on the threatened Westland petrel (Procellaria westlandica; Procellariidae), to investigate temporal patterns in colonial interactions, including vocalisations and social behaviours, in the context of petrel sociality. We conducted extensive behavioural observations on the colony at different time-scales (throughout single nights, between seasons, and across years) to characterise the temporal dynamics of at-colony
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Cirillo, Jasmin, and Dietmar Todt. "Perception and judgement of whispered vocalisations." Behaviour 142, no. 1 (2005): 113–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568539053627758.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Black-Décima, Patricia, Alejandra M. Hurtado, J. M. Barbanti Duarte, and Mirta Santana. "Acoustic parameters of courtship and human-directed friendly vocalisations in seven species of Neotropical deer in captivity." Animal Production Science 60, no. 10 (2020): 1283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1071/an19377.

Full text
Abstract:
Context Polygynous deer rut vocalisations have been found to be highly conspicuous, typical of the species and important in sexual selection. Information about vocalisations is not available for Neotropical species (subfamily Odocoileinae). Aims The objective was to record courtship vocalisations from all Neotropical deer available, looking for differences among species useful for taxonomic identification, characterise the vocalisations according to acoustic parameters and to try to determine functions. Methods Four species of brocket deer (red, Mazama americana (n = 9); grey, M. gouazoubira (
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Knudsen, Jan Sverre. "Children's Improvised Vocalisations: Learning, Communication and Technology of the Self." Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood 9, no. 4 (2008): 287–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.2304/ciec.2008.9.4.287.

Full text
Abstract:
The intention of this article is to explore, challenge and expand our understandings of children's improvised vocalisations, a fundamentally human form of expression. Based on selected examples from observation and recording in non-institutional settings, the article outlines how this phenomenon can be understood as learning and as communication. This is supplemented by suggesting a third possible approach which places these vocal forms within the frame of understanding implied by Foucault's term ‘technology of the self’. This theoretical perspective entails recognising improvised vocalisation
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Browne, Ciara Edan, Christine Erbe, and Robert D. McCauley. "Distribution and Seasonality of the Omura’s Whale (Balaenoptera omurai) in Australia Based on Passive Acoustic Recordings." Animals 14, no. 20 (2024): 2944. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani14202944.

Full text
Abstract:
The Omura’s whale (Balaenoptera omurai) is one of the most recently described species of baleen whale. Initially known only from stranding and whaling specimens, it has now been identified in all ocean basins excluding the central and eastern Pacific. Unlike most baleen whales that migrate between the poles and the equator seasonally, the Omura’s whale is known to inhabit tropical to sub-tropical waters year-round. In Australian waters, there remain fewer than 30 confirmed visual sightings over the past decade. However, based on acoustic records, the Omura’s whale has been detected off areas o
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Goddard, Cliff, and David Lambert. "Laughter, bonding and biological evolution." European Journal of Humour Research 10, no. 2 (2022): 14–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.7592/ejhr.2022.10.2.668.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper combines perspectives from evolutionary biology and linguistics to discuss the early evolution of laughter and the possible role of laughter-like vocalisation as a bonding mechanism in hominins and early human species. From the perspective of evolutionary biology, we here emphasise several things: the role of exaptation, the typically very slow pace of evolutionary change, and the danger of projecting backwards from the current utilities of laughter to infer its earlier function, hundreds of thousands, or even millions, of years ago. From the perspective of linguistics, we examine b
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Kim, Na Yeon, Seong Jin Kim, Se Young Jang, Hye Jin Seong, Yeong Sik Yun, and Sang Ho Moon. "Characteristics of vocalisation in Hanwoo cattle (Bos taurus coreanae) under different call-causing conditions." Animal Production Science 59, no. 12 (2019): 2169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1071/an17698.

Full text
Abstract:
The present study provided basic data regarding cattle farming on the basis of animal welfare, by using Hanwoo cattle (Bos taurus coreanae) vocalisations. We collected and analysed cattle-vocalisation data during situations that commonly occur during cattle farming (steer handling call, n = 130; heifer handling call, n = 418; heifer oestrus call, n = 454; heifer feed-anticipation call, n = 124; calf introduction call, n = 212) at a Hanwoo cattle farm and investigated differences in vocalisation parameters (call duration, call intensity, fundamental frequency, first formant, second formant, thi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Gordon, J. C. D., J. N. Matthews, S. Panigada, A. Gannier, J. F. Borsani, and G. Notarbartolo di Sciara. "Distribution and relative abundance of striped dolphins, and distribution of sperm whales in the Ligurian Sea cetacean sanctuary: results from a collaboration using acoustic monitoring techniques." J. Cetacean Res. Manage. 2, no. 1 (2000): 27–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.47536/jcrm.v2i1.486.

Full text
Abstract:
The distribution and relative abundance of groups of striped dolphins (Stenella coeruleoalba) in the Ligurian Sea cetacean sanctuary, based on acoustic surveys carried out in the summers of 1994-1996, is presented. Abundance indices based on acoustic detections were adjusted for covariates likely to influence the detectability of dolphin vocalisations, such as wind speed, background noise and sea state. Dolphin vocalisation rates were shown to vary diurnally, being higher at night, and this effect was also modelled and removed. Results showed that dolphin groups were fairly evenly distributed
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Neha Sharma and Amita Sarkar. "Study of Certain Behaviours of Red Wattled Lapwings in Sur Sarovar Bird Sanctuary." Journal of Science Innovations and Nature of Earth 5, no. 2 (2025): 15–17. https://doi.org/10.59436/jsiane.365.2583-2093.

Full text
Abstract:
Sur Sarovar Bird Sanctuary is a Ramsar site which is located within the Agra region of Uttar Pradesh, India. The Red Wattled Lapwing is a resident of South Asia and has distinct vocalisations and ground nesting habits. Field observations were conducted over a six-month period and data related to certain behaviours including feeding habits, courtship rituals and interaction with other species were collected. A finding suggests that Red Wattled Lapwing exhibits strong territorial behaviour with males actively defending nesting sites. The role of vocalisation is not just important in communicatio
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Walker, Peter. "Cross-Sensory Correspondences and Naive Conceptions of Natural Phenomena." Perception 41, no. 5 (2012): 620–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p7195.

Full text
Abstract:
Cross-sensory correspondences automatically intrude on performance in elaborate laboratory tasks (see Spence 2011 Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics73 971–995, for a review). Outside such tasks, might they be responsible for some popular misconceptions about natural phenomena? Four simple demonstrations reveal how the correspondences between surface-lightness and weight, and between surface-lightness and auditory pitch, generate misconceptions about the weight and movement of objects and the vocalisations of animals. Specifically, people expect darker objects to be heavier than lighter
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Peake, T. M., and P. K. McGregor. "Corncrake Crex crex census estimates: a conservation application of vocal individuality." Animal Biodiversity and Conservation 24, no. 1 (2001): 81–90. https://doi.org/10.32800/abc.2001.24.0081.

Full text
Abstract:
Vocal individuality could be used to estimate numbers of individuals in species otherwise difficult to monitor. However, the usefulness of this technique in providing conservation information is little studied. The vocalisations of the Corncrake show a high level of individual distinctiveness. This fact was used to examine current counting methods and estimate movement patterns within one breeding season. Information on individual identity gained from vocalisations increased census estimates by 20–30% and showed that male Corncrakes called less frequently than previous studies had suggested. M
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Poisbleau, Maud, Laurent Demongin, Loïc A. Hardouin, David Carslake, Marcel Eens, and Petra Quillfeldt. "Hatching Vocalisations in Free-Living Rockhopper Penguins." Ardea 101, no. 1 (2013): 39–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.5253/078.101.0105.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!