To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Vocalization.

Journal articles on the topic 'Vocalization'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Vocalization.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

DiMattina, Christopher, and Xiaoqin Wang. "Virtual Vocalization Stimuli for Investigating Neural Representations of Species-Specific Vocalizations." Journal of Neurophysiology 95, no. 2 (2006): 1244–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00818.2005.

Full text
Abstract:
Most studies investigating neural representations of species-specific vocalizations in non-human primates and other species have involved studying neural responses to vocalization tokens. One limitation of such approaches is the difficulty in determining which acoustical features of vocalizations evoke neural responses. Traditionally used filtering techniques are often inadequate in manipulating features of complex vocalizations. Furthermore, the use of vocalization tokens cannot fully account for intrinsic stochastic variations of vocalizations that are crucial in understanding the neural cod
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Yustian, Indra, Dedek Kurniawan, Zahrial Effendi, et al. "Vocalization of Western Tarsier (Cephalopachus bancanus Horsfield, 1821) in Bangka Island, Indonesia." Journal of Tropical Biodiversity and Biotechnology 6, no. 3 (2021): 65526. http://dx.doi.org/10.22146/jtbb.65526.

Full text
Abstract:
Every tarsier species performs different vocalization behaviour. Cephalopachus bancanus as one of the tarsier species listed as vulnerable in the IUCN red list has limited and different information about their vocalization. This research was designed to explore the species vocalization in the vicinity of Petaling Village, District of Bangka, Bangka Island, Indonesia. Tarsier vocalization inside temporary enclosures was recorded using a handy recorder and analysed using bioacoustics software Audacity 2.3.3 and Raven Pro 1.6.1. We described seven vocalization types with different functions and s
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Kim, Ho, and Seunghee Ha. "Relation between Early Vocalizations and Words." Communication Sciences & Disorders 27, no. 1 (2022): 1–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.12963/csd.22877.

Full text
Abstract:
Objectives: This study investigated the relationship of phonological characteristics between early vocalizations at 6-8 months, 12-14 months, and words at 18-20 months. Additionally, we aimed to identify which phonological characteristics of early vocalization can predict speech and language development at 18-20 months.Methods: Vocalizations were collected using Language ENvironmental Analysis (LENA) from 14 children at 6-8, 12-14, and 18-20 months. Vocalizations were classified as precanonical or canonical vocalization. Words were separated from the entire vocalizations at 18-20 months. Conso
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Eliades, Steven J., and Xiaoqin Wang. "Sensory-Motor Interaction in the Primate Auditory Cortex During Self-Initiated Vocalizations." Journal of Neurophysiology 89, no. 4 (2003): 2194–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00627.2002.

Full text
Abstract:
Little is known about sensory-motor interaction in the auditory cortex of primates at the level of single neurons and its role in supporting vocal communication. The present study investigated single-unit activities in the auditory cortex of a vocal primate, the common marmoset ( Callithrix jacchus), during self-initiated vocalizations. We found that 1) self-initiated vocalizations resulted in suppression of neural discharges in a majority of auditory cortical neurons. The vocalization-induced inhibition suppressed both spontaneous and stimulus-driven discharges. Suppressed units responded poo
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Heijmans, Shai. "About the 'Unreliability' of the Vocalization of Western Targum-Manuscripts." Aramaic Studies 9, no. 2 (2011): 279–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/147783511x619854.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The main argument for the unreliability of the Tiberian vocalization in Targum manuscripts of western origin is the inconsistency with which the vocalization signs are applied. The author argues that in certain manuscripts this inconsistency is the result of a non-Tiberian vocalization system which uses the Tiberian vocalization signs, the so-called Palestino-Tiberian vocalization system. A passage from an Ashkenazic Targumic manuscript with Palestino-Tiberian vocalization is examined and its 'inconsistencies' are explained in light of similar vocalizations in manuscripts of Rabbinic
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Eliades, Steven J., and Xiaoqin Wang. "Comparison of auditory-vocal interactions across multiple types of vocalizations in marmoset auditory cortex." Journal of Neurophysiology 109, no. 6 (2013): 1638–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00698.2012.

Full text
Abstract:
Auditory-vocal interaction, the modulation of auditory sensory responses during vocal production, is an important but poorly understood neurophysiological phenomenon in nonhuman primates. This sensory-motor processing has important behavioral implications for self-monitoring during vocal production as well as feedback-mediated vocal control for both animals and humans. Previous studies in marmosets have shown that a large portion of neurons in the auditory cortex are suppressed during self-produced vocalization but have primarily focused on a single type of isolation vocalization. The present
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Jeong, Yulim, and Seunghee Ha. "Temporal Characteristics of Infant Vocalization during Interaction with Adults." Communication Sciences & Disorders 29, no. 1 (2024): 107–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.12963/csd.240002.

Full text
Abstract:
Objectives: This study aimed to examine the temporal characteristics of infant vocalizations during interactions with adults in natural home environments.Methods: One day LENA (Language ENvironment Analysis) recordings were collected from 20 infants aged 6 and 12 months. The collected data were analyzed by selecting 20 5-mintue intervals in the order of highest CTCs (conversational turn counts). In the analysis unit of turn-taking, interactions in which the infant responded within 0.05 seconds to 3 seconds after the adult spoke directly to the infant were regarded as one analysis unit. The tem
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Lancaster, W. C., O. W. Henson, and A. W. Keating. "Respiratory muscle activity in relation to vocalization in flying bats." Journal of Experimental Biology 198, no. 1 (1995): 175–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.198.1.175.

Full text
Abstract:
The structure of the thoracic and abdominal walls of Pteronotus parnellii (Microchiroptera: Mormoopidae) was described with respect to their function in respiration and vocalization. We monitored electromyographic activity of respiratory and flight muscles in relation to echolocative vocalization. In flight, signals were telemetered with a small FM transmitter modified to summate the low-frequency myopotentials with biosonar signals from a ceramic-crystal microphone. Recordings were also made from the same bats confined to a small cage. Vocalizations were used as the parameter by which all mus
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

McCathren, Rebecca B., Paul J. Yoder, and Steven F. Warren. "The Relationship Between Prelinguistic Vocalization and Later Expressive Vocabulary in Young Children With Developmental Delay." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42, no. 4 (1999): 915–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4204.915.

Full text
Abstract:
This study tested the relationship between prelinguistic vocalization and expressive vocabulary 1 year later in young children with mild to moderate developmental delays. Three vocalization variables were tested: rate of all vocalization, rate of vocalizations with consonants, and rate of vocalizations used interactively. The 58 toddlers in the study were 17–34 months old, not sensory impaired, and had Bayley Mental Development Indices (Bayley, 1969; Bayley, 1993) from 35–85. In addition, the children had fewer than 3 words in their expressive vocabularies and during classroom observation each
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Czyżowski, Piotr, Sławomir Beeger, Mariusz Wójcik, Dorota Jarmoszczuk, Mirosław Karpiński, and Marian Flis. "Analysis of the Territorial Vocalization of the Pheasants Phasianus colchicus." Animals 12, no. 22 (2022): 3209. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani12223209.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of the study was to assess the impact of the duration of the mating season and the time of day on the parameters of the vocalization pheasants (duration of vocalization, frequency of the sound wave, intervals between vocalizations). In the study, pheasant vocalization recorded in the morning (600–800) and in the afternoon (1600–1800) between April and June 2020 was analyzed. In total, the research material consisted of 258 separate vocalizations. After recognition of the individual songs of each bird, frequency-time indicators were collected from the samples to perform statistical anal
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Feng, Min, Mengyao Zhai, Juncai Xu, et al. "Towards-Person Vocalization Effect on Screening for Autism Spectrum Disorders in the Context of Frustration." Brain Sciences 11, no. 12 (2021): 1651. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11121651.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this study is to investigate the vocalization characteristics of infants with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in the context of frustration. The duration and frequency of vocalization in 48 infants with ASD and 65 infants with typical development (TD) were followed up to 24 months later for subsequent diagnosis. The typical vocalizations of infants with ASD were retrospectively analyzed, such as speech-like vocalizations, nonspeech vocalizations, vocalizations towards the person and non-social vocalizations. The results showed that, compared with the TD group, vocalizations of in
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Raposo, Marcos A., and Elizabeth Höfling. "Overestimation of vocal characters in Suboscine taxonomy (Aves: Passeriformes: Tyranni): causes and implications." Lundiana: International Journal of Biodiversity 4, no. 1 (2022): 35–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.35699/2675-5327.2003.21833.

Full text
Abstract:
The difference in treatment of vocal features in Oscines and Suboscines passerine birds characterizes a large portion of the current studies on their taxonomy. In the former taxon, vocalization is supposed to be molded by learning, and consequently is not regarded as taxonomically informative. In the latter, a strong emphasis is given to vocalization because it supposedly reflects the genetic structure of populations. This paper reviews the various assumptions related to this difference in treatment, including the overestimation of the vocal characters in suboscine alpha taxonomy due to the al
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Clemmons, Janine R. "Development of a Selective Response To an Adult Vocalization in Nestling Black-Capped Chickadees." Behaviour 132, no. 1-2 (1995): 1–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853995x00252.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThere are many studies on how songbirds develop song production, but few on how songbirds develop appropriate responses to conspecific vocalizations. The black-capped chickadee, Parus atricapillus, produces a vocalization, the 'squawk', that stimulates gaping in nestlings during feeding. To determine whether nestlings gape selectively at the squawk, playbacks of several conspecific vocalizations plus a heterospecific vocalization were presented to nestlings within natural nests. A preference for the squawk did not appear until day 2-3 and then steadily increased, until by day 11-13, ne
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

THOMAS, Pieter, Tomasz GRZYWALSKI, Yuanbo HOU, et al. "Broiler chicken vocalization analysis during a medium-scale heat stress experiment." INTER-NOISE and NOISE-CON Congress and Conference Proceedings 270, no. 2 (2024): 9537–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.3397/in_2024_4262.

Full text
Abstract:
The poultry industry in Flanders, Belgium, is characterized by highly efficient production mechanisms. To improve awareness for animal health and welfare, the use of neural network-based broiler vocalization detectors has been recently proposed in scientific literature. However, these tools have not yet been tested when used continuously in large-scale deployments. This paper describes the deployment of the broiler vocalization detector to analyze the sonic environment during four experimental rounds, each containing 560 broilers, in a medium scale housing at ILVO, Belgium. Half the broilers w
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Huang, Ting, and Yurun Li. "Vocal Music Mispronunciation Identification and Correction Based on Artificial Intelligence Technology." Archives des Sciences 74, no. 4 (2024): 01–07. http://dx.doi.org/10.62227/as/74401.

Full text
Abstract:
In order to avoid blindness in vocal teaching, it is necessary to seek the correct method of vocalization that conforms to the principles of physiological science and to correct the wrong method of vocalization. In this paper, we describe the differences of vocal vocalizations by MFCC coefficients and pitch features, and adopt LAS model to extract high-level features in speech signals. And different vocalizations are explored in different acoustic feature parameter spaces. Deep learning and transcribed text techniques in artificial intelligence are utilized to detect and correct the errors ide
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Warkentin, Ian G., Heather E. Spicer, Jenna P. B. McDermott, Darroch M. Whitaker, and Erin E. Fraser. "Broadcasting American red squirrel vocalizations influences detection probability." PLOS ONE 20, no. 3 (2025): e0319351. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0319351.

Full text
Abstract:
Territorial responses by North American red squirrels (Tamiasciurus hudsonicus) to conspecifics vary seasonally with peaks during mating and dispersal periods. Broadcast of squirrel vocalizations during surveys may elicit territorial defense behaviors such as calling and movement that make individuals more available for detection, with implications for subsequent occupancy and abundance analyses. We examined the effect of vocalization broadcasts on detection probability during point counts throughout a 14-month period at two locations (year-round study) and during two summers at a third locati
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Calvert, Wendy, and Ian Stirling. "Winter Distribution of Ringed Seals (Phoca hispida) in the Barrow Strait Area, Northwest Territories, Determined by Underwater Vocalizations." Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 42, no. 7 (1985): 1238–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/f85-153.

Full text
Abstract:
In order to assess underice distribution of ringed seals (Phoca hispida) in winter, we made recordings from 23 to 30 April 1982 at 32 sites chosen to represent different habitats in the High Arctic. By regressing the vocalization rate at each site against variables for habitat quality, we found that sites in smooth interisland channels had significantly more vocalizations than sites in bays, and sites with frequent human activity had vocalization rates similar to the overall average. Although differences in vocalization rates correlated with some measured and estimated habitat variables, there
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Job, Damon A., Daryl J. Boness, and John M. Francis. "Individual variation in nursing vocalizations of Hawaiian monk seal pups, Monachus schauinslandi (Phocidae, Pinnipedia), and lack of maternal recognition." Canadian Journal of Zoology 73, no. 5 (1995): 975–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/z95-114.

Full text
Abstract:
Vocalizations of individual Hawaiian monk seal pups, Monachus schauinslandi, do not have unique attributes that enable females to recognize their own offspring. Despite low aggregation density during pupping, aggressive encounters are common between females with pups. Fostering is prevalent and may reflect confusion over the identity of pups following aggressive encounters between females. All pup vocalizations were simple in structure and contained true harmonics. The coefficients of variation revealed considerable variance in vocalization structure within pups. Controlling for age, multivari
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Ha, Seunghee, and Rayeon Lee. "Adult Verbal Responses Facilitating Child Vocalizations." Communication Sciences & Disorders 29, no. 2 (2024): 462–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.12963/csd.240033.

Full text
Abstract:
Objectives: This study aimed to investigate whether adults in the vicinity of children respond differently to various child vocalization types within three-event sequences of “child vocalization-adult verbal response-child vocalization” occurring in everyday life. Additionally, this study examined the types of adult verbal responses that facilitate child vocalizations within the series of three-event sequential interactions between children and adults. Methods: Data was collected using the LENA (Language ENvironment Analysis) from 30 children aged 12 to 18 months and their families. Three-even
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Perlman, Marcus, and Ashley A. Cain. "Iconicity in vocalization, comparisons with gesture, and implications for theories on the evolution of language." Gesture 14, no. 3 (2014): 320–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/gest.14.3.03per.

Full text
Abstract:
Scholars have often reasoned that vocalizations are extremely limited in their potential for iconic expression, especially in comparison to manual gestures (e.g., Armstrong & Wilcox, 2007; Tomasello, 2008). As evidence for an alternative view, we first review the growing body of research related to iconicity in vocalizations, including experimental work on sound symbolism, cross-linguistic studies documenting iconicity in the grammars and lexicons of languages, and experimental studies that examine iconicity in the production of speech and vocalizations. We then report an experiment in whi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Van Parijs, Sofie M., and Kit M. Kovacs. "In-air and underwater vocalizations of eastern Canadian harbour seals, Phoca vitulina." Canadian Journal of Zoology 80, no. 7 (2002): 1173–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/z02-088.

Full text
Abstract:
Harbour seals, Phoca vitulina, have long been thought to be one of the least vocal pinniped species both in air and under water. However, recent studies have shown that males use underwater vocalizations intensively during the mating season. In air, harbour seals are still thought to be relatively silent. In this study we describe the vocal repertoire of Eastern Canadian harbour seals during the breeding season. Harbour seals from this area produced seven vocalization types in air and one vocalization type under water. In-air vocalizations are predominantly used by adult males during agonistic
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

West, R. A., and C. R. Larson. "Neurons of the anterior mesial cortex related to faciovocal activity in the awake monkey." Journal of Neurophysiology 74, no. 5 (1995): 1856–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1995.74.5.1856.

Full text
Abstract:
1. The anterior mesial cortex, including the cingulate region, is thought to be involved in the voluntary control of vocalization. Previous recording studies have demonstrated that anterior mesial neurons discharge before conditioned and spontaneous vocalizations, but questions remain regarding the location and functional properties of these neurons. The present study was performed to provide a more complete description of the location and discharge properties of anterior mesial neurons involved in faciovocal behaviors. 2. Single-unit activity was recorded from neurons in the anterior mesial c
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Dilley, Laura, and Derek Houston. "Accuracy of the Language Environment Analysis (LENA) speech processing system in identifying communicative vocalizations of young children and adults." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 150, no. 4 (2021): A358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/10.0008584.

Full text
Abstract:
The Language Environment Analysis (LENA) system is an automated audio processing system widely used for characterizing language behaviors of children and adults for clinical and basic research. While a number of studies have assessed LENA’s reliability, its accuracy at identifying and counting speech communicative events is still not well-characterized under a range of naturalistic conditions. In two studies, we examined accuracy of LENA's speech vocalization classifications, relative to human gold standard coding for audio events, as well as word and speech vocalization counts for adults and
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Wang, X., M. M. Merzenich, R. Beitel, and C. E. Schreiner. "Representation of a species-specific vocalization in the primary auditory cortex of the common marmoset: temporal and spectral characteristics." Journal of Neurophysiology 74, no. 6 (1995): 2685–706. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1995.74.6.2685.

Full text
Abstract:
1. The temporal and spectral characteristics of neural representations of a behaviorally important species-specific vocalization were studied in neuronal populations of the primary auditory cortex (A1) of barbiturate-anesthetized adult common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), using both natural and synthetic vocalizations. The natural vocalizations used in electrophysiological experiments were recorded from the animals under study or from their conspecifics. These calls were frequently produced in vocal exchanges between members of our marmoset colony and are part of the well-defined and highly
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

DeVeney, Shari L., Anastasia Kyvelidou, and Paris Mather. "A home-based longitudinal study of vocalization behaviors across infants at low and elevated risk of autism." Autism & Developmental Language Impairments 6 (January 2021): 239694152110576. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/23969415211057658.

Full text
Abstract:
Background and Aims: The purpose of this exploratory study was to expand existing literature on prelinguistic vocalizations by reporting results of the first home-based longitudinal study examining a wide variety of behaviors and characteristics, including early vocalizations, across infants at low and elevated risk of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The study of vocalizations and vocalization changes across early developmental periods shows promise in reflecting early clinically significant differences across infants at low and elevated risk of ASD. Observations of early vocalizations and the
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Andreychev, Alexey. "Vocal interaction between Tawny Owl (Strix aluco) and Ural Owl (Strix uralensis) in the Middle Volga." E3S Web of Conferences 510 (2024): 03011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1051/e3sconf/202451003011.

Full text
Abstract:
The work studied the co-vocalization of Tawny Owl and Ural Owl. Researched primarily using digital voice recorders. Co-vocalization was revealed only in spring. This is due to the breeding season of birds. The roll call of two species of owls was recorded 2 h 30 min after sunset. Co-vocalization was always observed from 21:00 to 22:00 hours. Duration of co-vocalization of the Tawny Owl and the Ural Owl ranged from several sec to 6 min. During periods of co-vocalization, the Tawny Owl managed to produce twice as many calls as the Ural Owl. The Tawny Owl was the first to start calling, and later
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Seidl, Amanda, Alejandrina Cristia, Melanie Soderstrom, et al. "Infant–Mother Acoustic–Prosodic Alignment and Developmental Risk." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 61, no. 6 (2018): 1369–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2018_jslhr-s-17-0287.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose One promising early marker for autism and other communicative and language disorders is early infant speech production. Here we used daylong recordings of high- and low-risk infant–mother dyads to examine whether acoustic–prosodic alignment as well as two automated measures of infant vocalization are related to developmental risk status indexed via familial risk and developmental progress at 36 months of age. Method Automated analyses of the acoustics of daylong real-world interactions were used to examine whether pitch characteristics of one vocalization by the mother or the child pre
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Stirling, Ian, Wendy Calvert, and Cheryl Spencer. "Evidence of stereotyped underwater vocalizations of male Atlantic walruses (Odobenus rosmarus rosmarus)." Canadian Journal of Zoology 65, no. 9 (1987): 2311–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/z87-348.

Full text
Abstract:
Adult male Atlantic walruses (Odobenus rosmarus rosmarus) vocalize extensively underwater during the breeding season. The individual calls are composed of one or more short repetitious pulses which may vary individually in the number, pattern, and rate at which they are given. Individual male walruses give repeated stereotyped vocalization cycles totalling several hundred pulses each for up to several hours at a time, both while the whole body is submerged and between breaths with the head submerged while at the surface. We analyzed the vocalization cycles of a sample of different walruses, an
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Volker Glöckner, and Otto Von Helversen. "The Vocal Repertoire of Two Sympatric Species of Nectar-Feeding Bats (Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi)." Acta Chiropterologica 12, no. 1 (2010): 205–15. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13409346.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic si
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Volker Glöckner, and Otto Von Helversen. "The Vocal Repertoire of Two Sympatric Species of Nectar-Feeding Bats ( Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi )." Acta Chiropterologica 12, no. 1 (2010): 205–15. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13519511.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic si
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Volker Glöckner, and Otto Von Helversen. "The Vocal Repertoire of Two Sympatric Species of Nectar-Feeding Bats (Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi)." Acta Chiropterologica 12, no. 1 (2010): 205–15. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13409346.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic si
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Volker Glöckner, and Otto Von Helversen. "The Vocal Repertoire of Two Sympatric Species of Nectar-Feeding Bats ( Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi )." Acta Chiropterologica 12, no. 1 (2010): 205–15. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13519511.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic si
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Volker Glöckner, and Otto Von Helversen. "The Vocal Repertoire of Two Sympatric Species of Nectar-Feeding Bats (Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi)." Acta Chiropterologica 12, no. 1 (2010): 205–15. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13409346.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic si
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Volker Glöckner, and Otto Von Helversen. "The Vocal Repertoire of Two Sympatric Species of Nectar-Feeding Bats (Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi)." Acta Chiropterologica 12, no. 1 (2010): 205–15. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13409346.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic si
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Volker Glöckner, and Otto Von Helversen. "The Vocal Repertoire of Two Sympatric Species of Nectar-Feeding Bats (Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi)." Acta Chiropterologica 12, no. 1 (2010): 205–15. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13409346.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic si
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Volker Glöckner, and Otto Von Helversen. "The Vocal Repertoire of Two Sympatric Species of Nectar-Feeding Bats ( Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi )." Acta Chiropterologica 12, no. 1 (2010): 205–15. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13519511.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic si
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Volker Glöckner, and Otto Von Helversen. "The Vocal Repertoire of Two Sympatric Species of Nectar-Feeding Bats (Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi)." Acta Chiropterologica 12, no. 1 (2010): 205–15. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13409346.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic si
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Volker Glöckner, and Otto Von Helversen. "The Vocal Repertoire of Two Sympatric Species of Nectar-Feeding Bats ( Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi )." Acta Chiropterologica 12, no. 1 (2010): 205–15. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13519511.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic si
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Volker Glöckner, and Otto Von Helversen. "The Vocal Repertoire of Two Sympatric Species of Nectar-Feeding Bats (Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi)." Acta Chiropterologica 12, no. 1 (2010): 205–15. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13409346.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic si
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Romanski, Lizabeth M., Bruno B. Averbeck, and Mark Diltz. "Neural Representation of Vocalizations in the Primate Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex." Journal of Neurophysiology 93, no. 2 (2005): 734–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00675.2004.

Full text
Abstract:
In this study, we examined the role of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in encoding communication stimuli. Specifically, we recorded single-unit responses from the ventrolateral prefrontal cortext (vlPFC) in awake behaving rhesus macaques in response to species-specific vocalizations. We determined the selectivity of vlPFC cells for 10 types of rhesus vocalizations and also asked what types of vocalizations cluster together in the neuronal response. The data from the present study demonstrate that vlPFC auditory neurons respond to a variety of species-specific vocalizations from a previousl
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Mohebbi-Kalkhoran, Hamed, Shourav Pednekar, Chenyang Zhu, et al. "Passive ocean acoustic waveguide remote sensing of vocalization behavior and spatial distribution of diverse marine mammal species in the Norwegian and Barents Sea." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 154, no. 4_supplement (2023): A132. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/10.0023025.

Full text
Abstract:
Leveraging data acquired using a 160-element coherent hydrophone array deployed in the Norwegian and Barents Seas during spring 2014, and the passive ocean acoustic waveguide remote sensing (POAWRS) technique is employed to enable instantaneous wide-area monitoring of marine mammal vocalizations over expanses exceeding 100 km in diameter. The vocalization behavior of diverse marine mammal species including Fin, Humpback, Minke, Sperm, and Beluga whales are analyzed, quantifying time-frequency characteristics and call patterns from their vocalization signals present in high-resolution beamforme
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Cao, Dandan, Hong Zhou, Wei Wei, et al. "Vocal repertoire of adult captive red pandas (Ailurus fulgens)." Animal Biology 66, no. 2 (2016): 145–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15707563-00002493.

Full text
Abstract:
Vocal signals are a common communication tool used to recognize different individuals, advertise fertile phases or discriminate amongst potential mates. Therefore, a thorough understanding of vocal repertoires forms the basis for investigating the role of acoustic signaling in the sexual and social behavior of any animal. Red pandas (Ailurus fulgens) are classified as a vulnerable species and have declined by as much as 40% over the past 50 years in China. Adult red pandas are known to call frequently during mating and aggressive encounters; however, no quantitative description of their vocali
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Scheerer, Nichole E., Anupreet K. Tumber, and Jeffery A. Jones. "Attentional demands modulate sensorimotor learning induced by persistent exposure to changes in auditory feedback." Journal of Neurophysiology 115, no. 2 (2016): 826–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00799.2015.

Full text
Abstract:
Hearing one's own voice is important for regulating ongoing speech and for mapping speech sounds onto articulator movements. However, it is currently unknown whether attention mediates changes in the relationship between motor commands and their acoustic output, which are necessary as growth and aging inevitably cause changes to the vocal tract. In this study, participants produced vocalizations while they heard their vocal pitch persistently shifted downward one semitone in both single- and dual-task conditions. During the single-task condition, participants vocalized while passively viewing
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Ramsdell-Hudock, Heather L., Anne S. Warlaumont, Lindsey E. Foss, and Candice Perry. "Classification of Infant Vocalizations by Untrained Listeners." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 62, no. 9 (2019): 3265–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2019_jslhr-s-18-0494.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose To better enable communication among researchers, clinicians, and caregivers, we aimed to assess how untrained listeners classify early infant vocalization types in comparison to terms currently used by researchers and clinicians. Method Listeners were caregivers with no prior formal education in speech and language development. A 1st group of listeners reported on clinician/researcher-classified vowel, squeal, growl, raspberry, whisper, laugh, and cry vocalizations obtained from archived video/audio recordings of 10 infants from 4 through 12 months of age. A list of commonly used term
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Trösch, Cuzol, Parias, Calandreau, Nowak, and Lansade. "Horses Categorize Human Emotions Cross-Modally Based on Facial Expression and Non-Verbal Vocalizations." Animals 9, no. 11 (2019): 862. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani9110862.

Full text
Abstract:
Over the last few years, an increasing number of studies have aimed to gain more insight into the field of animal emotions. In particular, it is of interest to determine whether animals can cross-modally categorize the emotions of others. For domestic animals that share a close relationship with humans, we might wonder whether this cross-modal recognition of emotions extends to humans, as well. In this study, we tested whether horses could recognize human emotions and attribute the emotional valence of visual (facial expression) and vocal (non-verbal vocalization) stimuli to the same perceptua
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Burchardt, Lara S., Philipp Norton, Oliver Behr, Constance Scharff, and Mirjam Knörnschild. "General isochronous rhythm in echolocation calls and social vocalizations of the bat Saccopteryx bilineata." Royal Society Open Science 6, no. 1 (2019): 181076. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.181076.

Full text
Abstract:
Rhythm is an essential component of human speech and music but very little is known about its evolutionary origin and its distribution in animal vocalizations. We found a regular rhythm in three multisyllabic vocalization types (echolocation call sequences, male territorial songs and pup isolation calls) of the neotropical bat Saccopteryx bilineata . The intervals between element onsets were used to fit the rhythm for each individual. For echolocation call sequences, we expected rhythm frequencies around 6–24 Hz, corresponding to the wingbeat in S. bilineata which is strongly coupled to echolo
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Burchardt, Lara S., Philipp Norton, Oliver Behr, Constance Scharff, and Mirjam Knörnschild. "General isochronous rhythm in echolocation calls and social vocalizations of the bat Saccopteryx bilineata." Royal Society Open Science 6, no. 1 (2019): 181076. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13439918.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) Rhythm is an essential component of human speech and music but very little is known about its evolutionary origin and its distribution in animal vocalizations. We found a regular rhythm in three multisyllabic vocalization types (echolocation call sequences, male territorial songs and pup isolation calls) of the neotropical bat Saccopteryx bilineata . The intervals between element onsets were used to fit the rhythm for each individual. For echolocation call sequences, we expected rhythm frequencies around 6–24 Hz, corresponding to the wingbeat
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Oliver Behr, and Helversen Otto Von. "Babbling behavior in the sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata)." Naturwissenschaften 93, no. 9 (2006): 451–54. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13448715.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) Infant babbling in humans and a few other primates plays an important role in allowing the young to practice the adult vocal repertoire during early behavioral development. Vocalizations uttered during babbling resemble, to some degree, the acoustic structure of adult vocalizations and are often produced in long bouts independent of any social context. Similar behavior, termed subsong or plastic song, is known from a variety of songbirds. Here, we show that pups of the sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata), a species with an unusually large v
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Burchardt, Lara S., Philipp Norton, Oliver Behr, Constance Scharff, and Mirjam Knörnschild. "General isochronous rhythm in echolocation calls and social vocalizations of the bat Saccopteryx bilineata." Royal Society Open Science 6, no. 1 (2019): 181076. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13439918.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) Rhythm is an essential component of human speech and music but very little is known about its evolutionary origin and its distribution in animal vocalizations. We found a regular rhythm in three multisyllabic vocalization types (echolocation call sequences, male territorial songs and pup isolation calls) of the neotropical bat Saccopteryx bilineata . The intervals between element onsets were used to fit the rhythm for each individual. For echolocation call sequences, we expected rhythm frequencies around 6–24 Hz, corresponding to the wingbeat
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Knörnschild, Mirjam, Oliver Behr, and Helversen Otto Von. "Babbling behavior in the sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata)." Naturwissenschaften 93, no. 9 (2006): 451–54. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13448715.

Full text
Abstract:
(Uploaded by Plazi for the Bat Literature Project) Infant babbling in humans and a few other primates plays an important role in allowing the young to practice the adult vocal repertoire during early behavioral development. Vocalizations uttered during babbling resemble, to some degree, the acoustic structure of adult vocalizations and are often produced in long bouts independent of any social context. Similar behavior, termed subsong or plastic song, is known from a variety of songbirds. Here, we show that pups of the sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata), a species with an unusually large v
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!